Logo for College of DuPage Digital Press

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

7 Module 7: Thinking, Reasoning, and Problem-Solving

This module is about how a solid working knowledge of psychological principles can help you to think more effectively, so you can succeed in school and life. You might be inclined to believe that—because you have been thinking for as long as you can remember, because you are able to figure out the solution to many problems, because you feel capable of using logic to argue a point, because you can evaluate whether the things you read and hear make sense—you do not need any special training in thinking. But this, of course, is one of the key barriers to helping people think better. If you do not believe that there is anything wrong, why try to fix it?

The human brain is indeed a remarkable thinking machine, capable of amazing, complex, creative, logical thoughts. Why, then, are we telling you that you need to learn how to think? Mainly because one major lesson from cognitive psychology is that these capabilities of the human brain are relatively infrequently realized. Many psychologists believe that people are essentially “cognitive misers.” It is not that we are lazy, but that we have a tendency to expend the least amount of mental effort necessary. Although you may not realize it, it actually takes a great deal of energy to think. Careful, deliberative reasoning and critical thinking are very difficult. Because we seem to be successful without going to the trouble of using these skills well, it feels unnecessary to develop them. As you shall see, however, there are many pitfalls in the cognitive processes described in this module. When people do not devote extra effort to learning and improving reasoning, problem solving, and critical thinking skills, they make many errors.

As is true for memory, if you develop the cognitive skills presented in this module, you will be more successful in school. It is important that you realize, however, that these skills will help you far beyond school, even more so than a good memory will. Although it is somewhat useful to have a good memory, ten years from now no potential employer will care how many questions you got right on multiple choice exams during college. All of them will, however, recognize whether you are a logical, analytical, critical thinker. With these thinking skills, you will be an effective, persuasive communicator and an excellent problem solver.

The module begins by describing different kinds of thought and knowledge, especially conceptual knowledge and critical thinking. An understanding of these differences will be valuable as you progress through school and encounter different assignments that require you to tap into different kinds of knowledge. The second section covers deductive and inductive reasoning, which are processes we use to construct and evaluate strong arguments. They are essential skills to have whenever you are trying to persuade someone (including yourself) of some point, or to respond to someone’s efforts to persuade you. The module ends with a section about problem solving. A solid understanding of the key processes involved in problem solving will help you to handle many daily challenges.

7.1. Different kinds of thought

7.2. Reasoning and Judgment

7.3. Problem Solving

READING WITH PURPOSE

Remember and understand.

By reading and studying Module 7, you should be able to remember and describe:

  • Concepts and inferences (7.1)
  • Procedural knowledge (7.1)
  • Metacognition (7.1)
  • Characteristics of critical thinking:  skepticism; identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions; reasoning and problem solving skills  (7.1)
  • Reasoning:  deductive reasoning, deductively valid argument, inductive reasoning, inductively strong argument, availability heuristic, representativeness heuristic  (7.2)
  • Fixation:  functional fixedness, mental set  (7.3)
  • Algorithms, heuristics, and the role of confirmation bias (7.3)
  • Effective problem solving sequence (7.3)

By reading and thinking about how the concepts in Module 6 apply to real life, you should be able to:

  • Identify which type of knowledge a piece of information is (7.1)
  • Recognize examples of deductive and inductive reasoning (7.2)
  • Recognize judgments that have probably been influenced by the availability heuristic (7.2)
  • Recognize examples of problem solving heuristics and algorithms (7.3)

Analyze, Evaluate, and Create

By reading and thinking about Module 6, participating in classroom activities, and completing out-of-class assignments, you should be able to:

  • Use the principles of critical thinking to evaluate information (7.1)
  • Explain whether examples of reasoning arguments are deductively valid or inductively strong (7.2)
  • Outline how you could try to solve a problem from your life using the effective problem solving sequence (7.3)

7.1. Different kinds of thought and knowledge

  • Take a few minutes to write down everything that you know about dogs.
  • Do you believe that:
  • Psychic ability exists?
  • Hypnosis is an altered state of consciousness?
  • Magnet therapy is effective for relieving pain?
  • Aerobic exercise is an effective treatment for depression?
  • UFO’s from outer space have visited earth?

On what do you base your belief or disbelief for the questions above?

Of course, we all know what is meant by the words  think  and  knowledge . You probably also realize that they are not unitary concepts; there are different kinds of thought and knowledge. In this section, let us look at some of these differences. If you are familiar with these different kinds of thought and pay attention to them in your classes, it will help you to focus on the right goals, learn more effectively, and succeed in school. Different assignments and requirements in school call on you to use different kinds of knowledge or thought, so it will be very helpful for you to learn to recognize them (Anderson, et al. 2001).

Factual and conceptual knowledge

Module 5 introduced the idea of declarative memory, which is composed of facts and episodes. If you have ever played a trivia game or watched Jeopardy on TV, you realize that the human brain is able to hold an extraordinary number of facts. Likewise, you realize that each of us has an enormous store of episodes, essentially facts about events that happened in our own lives. It may be difficult to keep that in mind when we are struggling to retrieve one of those facts while taking an exam, however. Part of the problem is that, in contradiction to the advice from Module 5, many students continue to try to memorize course material as a series of unrelated facts (picture a history student simply trying to memorize history as a set of unrelated dates without any coherent story tying them together). Facts in the real world are not random and unorganized, however. It is the way that they are organized that constitutes a second key kind of knowledge, conceptual.

Concepts are nothing more than our mental representations of categories of things in the world. For example, think about dogs. When you do this, you might remember specific facts about dogs, such as they have fur and they bark. You may also recall dogs that you have encountered and picture them in your mind. All of this information (and more) makes up your concept of dog. You can have concepts of simple categories (e.g., triangle), complex categories (e.g., small dogs that sleep all day, eat out of the garbage, and bark at leaves), kinds of people (e.g., psychology professors), events (e.g., birthday parties), and abstract ideas (e.g., justice). Gregory Murphy (2002) refers to concepts as the “glue that holds our mental life together” (p. 1). Very simply, summarizing the world by using concepts is one of the most important cognitive tasks that we do. Our conceptual knowledge  is  our knowledge about the world. Individual concepts are related to each other to form a rich interconnected network of knowledge. For example, think about how the following concepts might be related to each other: dog, pet, play, Frisbee, chew toy, shoe. Or, of more obvious use to you now, how these concepts are related: working memory, long-term memory, declarative memory, procedural memory, and rehearsal? Because our minds have a natural tendency to organize information conceptually, when students try to remember course material as isolated facts, they are working against their strengths.

One last important point about concepts is that they allow you to instantly know a great deal of information about something. For example, if someone hands you a small red object and says, “here is an apple,” they do not have to tell you, “it is something you can eat.” You already know that you can eat it because it is true by virtue of the fact that the object is an apple; this is called drawing an  inference , assuming that something is true on the basis of your previous knowledge (for example, of category membership or of how the world works) or logical reasoning.

Procedural knowledge

Physical skills, such as tying your shoes, doing a cartwheel, and driving a car (or doing all three at the same time, but don’t try this at home) are certainly a kind of knowledge. They are procedural knowledge, the same idea as procedural memory that you saw in Module 5. Mental skills, such as reading, debating, and planning a psychology experiment, are procedural knowledge, as well. In short, procedural knowledge is the knowledge how to do something (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993).

Metacognitive knowledge

Floyd used to think that he had a great memory. Now, he has a better memory. Why? Because he finally realized that his memory was not as great as he once thought it was. Because Floyd eventually learned that he often forgets where he put things, he finally developed the habit of putting things in the same place. (Unfortunately, he did not learn this lesson before losing at least 5 watches and a wedding ring.) Because he finally realized that he often forgets to do things, he finally started using the To Do list app on his phone. And so on. Floyd’s insights about the real limitations of his memory have allowed him to remember things that he used to forget.

All of us have knowledge about the way our own minds work. You may know that you have a good memory for people’s names and a poor memory for math formulas. Someone else might realize that they have difficulty remembering to do things, like stopping at the store on the way home. Others still know that they tend to overlook details. This knowledge about our own thinking is actually quite important; it is called metacognitive knowledge, or  metacognition . Like other kinds of thinking skills, it is subject to error. For example, in unpublished research, one of the authors surveyed about 120 General Psychology students on the first day of the term. Among other questions, the students were asked them to predict their grade in the class and report their current Grade Point Average. Two-thirds of the students predicted that their grade in the course would be higher than their GPA. (The reality is that at our college, students tend to earn lower grades in psychology than their overall GPA.) Another example: Students routinely report that they thought they had done well on an exam, only to discover, to their dismay, that they were wrong (more on that important problem in a moment). Both errors reveal a breakdown in metacognition.

The Dunning-Kruger Effect

In general, most college students probably do not study enough. For example, using data from the National Survey of Student Engagement, Fosnacht, McCormack, and Lerma (2018) reported that first-year students at 4-year colleges in the U.S. averaged less than 14 hours per week preparing for classes. The typical suggestion is that you should spend two hours outside of class for every hour in class, or 24 – 30 hours per week for a full-time student. Clearly, students in general are nowhere near that recommended mark. Many observers, including some faculty, believe that this shortfall is a result of students being too busy or lazy. Now, it may be true that many students are too busy, with work and family obligations, for example. Others, are not particularly motivated in school, and therefore might correctly be labeled lazy. A third possible explanation, however, is that some students might not think they need to spend this much time. And this is a matter of metacognition. Consider the scenario that we mentioned above, students thinking they had done well on an exam only to discover that they did not. Justin Kruger and David Dunning examined scenarios very much like this in 1999. Kruger and Dunning gave research participants tests measuring humor, logic, and grammar. Then, they asked the participants to assess their own abilities and test performance in these areas. They found that participants in general tended to overestimate their abilities, already a problem with metacognition. Importantly, the participants who scored the lowest overestimated their abilities the most. Specifically, students who scored in the bottom quarter (averaging in the 12th percentile) thought they had scored in the 62nd percentile. This has become known as the  Dunning-Kruger effect . Many individual faculty members have replicated these results with their own student on their course exams, including the authors of this book. Think about it. Some students who just took an exam and performed poorly believe that they did well before seeing their score. It seems very likely that these are the very same students who stopped studying the night before because they thought they were “done.” Quite simply, it is not just that they did not know the material. They did not know that they did not know the material. That is poor metacognition.

In order to develop good metacognitive skills, you should continually monitor your thinking and seek frequent feedback on the accuracy of your thinking (Medina, Castleberry, & Persky 2017). For example, in classes get in the habit of predicting your exam grades. As soon as possible after taking an exam, try to find out which questions you missed and try to figure out why. If you do this soon enough, you may be able to recall the way it felt when you originally answered the question. Did you feel confident that you had answered the question correctly? Then you have just discovered an opportunity to improve your metacognition. Be on the lookout for that feeling and respond with caution.

concept :  a mental representation of a category of things in the world

Dunning-Kruger effect : individuals who are less competent tend to overestimate their abilities more than individuals who are more competent do

inference : an assumption about the truth of something that is not stated. Inferences come from our prior knowledge and experience, and from logical reasoning

metacognition :  knowledge about one’s own cognitive processes; thinking about your thinking

Critical thinking

One particular kind of knowledge or thinking skill that is related to metacognition is  critical thinking (Chew, 2020). You may have noticed that critical thinking is an objective in many college courses, and thus it could be a legitimate topic to cover in nearly any college course. It is particularly appropriate in psychology, however. As the science of (behavior and) mental processes, psychology is obviously well suited to be the discipline through which you should be introduced to this important way of thinking.

More importantly, there is a particular need to use critical thinking in psychology. We are all, in a way, experts in human behavior and mental processes, having engaged in them literally since birth. Thus, perhaps more than in any other class, students typically approach psychology with very clear ideas and opinions about its subject matter. That is, students already “know” a lot about psychology. The problem is, “it ain’t so much the things we don’t know that get us into trouble. It’s the things we know that just ain’t so” (Ward, quoted in Gilovich 1991). Indeed, many of students’ preconceptions about psychology are just plain wrong. Randolph Smith (2002) wrote a book about critical thinking in psychology called  Challenging Your Preconceptions,  highlighting this fact. On the other hand, many of students’ preconceptions about psychology are just plain right! But wait, how do you know which of your preconceptions are right and which are wrong? And when you come across a research finding or theory in this class that contradicts your preconceptions, what will you do? Will you stick to your original idea, discounting the information from the class? Will you immediately change your mind? Critical thinking can help us sort through this confusing mess.

But what is critical thinking? The goal of critical thinking is simple to state (but extraordinarily difficult to achieve): it is to be right, to draw the correct conclusions, to believe in things that are true and to disbelieve things that are false. We will provide two definitions of critical thinking (or, if you like, one large definition with two distinct parts). First, a more conceptual one: Critical thinking is thinking like a scientist in your everyday life (Schmaltz, Jansen, & Wenckowski, 2017).  Our second definition is more operational; it is simply a list of skills that are essential to be a critical thinker. Critical thinking entails solid reasoning and problem solving skills; skepticism; and an ability to identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions. Excellent deductive and inductive reasoning, and problem solving skills contribute to critical thinking. So, you can consider the subject matter of sections 7.2 and 7.3 to be part of critical thinking. Because we will be devoting considerable time to these concepts in the rest of the module, let us begin with a discussion about the other aspects of critical thinking.

Let’s address that first part of the definition. Scientists form hypotheses, or predictions about some possible future observations. Then, they collect data, or information (think of this as making those future observations). They do their best to make unbiased observations using reliable techniques that have been verified by others. Then, and only then, they draw a conclusion about what those observations mean. Oh, and do not forget the most important part. “Conclusion” is probably not the most appropriate word because this conclusion is only tentative. A scientist is always prepared that someone else might come along and produce new observations that would require a new conclusion be drawn. Wow! If you like to be right, you could do a lot worse than using a process like this.

A Critical Thinker’s Toolkit 

Now for the second part of the definition. Good critical thinkers (and scientists) rely on a variety of tools to evaluate information. Perhaps the most recognizable tool for critical thinking is  skepticism (and this term provides the clearest link to the thinking like a scientist definition, as you are about to see). Some people intend it as an insult when they call someone a skeptic. But if someone calls you a skeptic, if they are using the term correctly, you should consider it a great compliment. Simply put, skepticism is a way of thinking in which you refrain from drawing a conclusion or changing your mind until good evidence has been provided. People from Missouri should recognize this principle, as Missouri is known as the Show-Me State. As a skeptic, you are not inclined to believe something just because someone said so, because someone else believes it, or because it sounds reasonable. You must be persuaded by high quality evidence.

Of course, if that evidence is produced, you have a responsibility as a skeptic to change your belief. Failure to change a belief in the face of good evidence is not skepticism; skepticism has open mindedness at its core. M. Neil Browne and Stuart Keeley (2018) use the term weak sense critical thinking to describe critical thinking behaviors that are used only to strengthen a prior belief. Strong sense critical thinking, on the other hand, has as its goal reaching the best conclusion. Sometimes that means strengthening your prior belief, but sometimes it means changing your belief to accommodate the better evidence.

Many times, a failure to think critically or weak sense critical thinking is related to a  bias , an inclination, tendency, leaning, or prejudice. Everybody has biases, but many people are unaware of them. Awareness of your own biases gives you the opportunity to control or counteract them. Unfortunately, however, many people are happy to let their biases creep into their attempts to persuade others; indeed, it is a key part of their persuasive strategy. To see how these biases influence messages, just look at the different descriptions and explanations of the same events given by people of different ages or income brackets, or conservative versus liberal commentators, or by commentators from different parts of the world. Of course, to be successful, these people who are consciously using their biases must disguise them. Even undisguised biases can be difficult to identify, so disguised ones can be nearly impossible.

Here are some common sources of biases:

  • Personal values and beliefs.  Some people believe that human beings are basically driven to seek power and that they are typically in competition with one another over scarce resources. These beliefs are similar to the world-view that political scientists call “realism.” Other people believe that human beings prefer to cooperate and that, given the chance, they will do so. These beliefs are similar to the world-view known as “idealism.” For many people, these deeply held beliefs can influence, or bias, their interpretations of such wide ranging situations as the behavior of nations and their leaders or the behavior of the driver in the car ahead of you. For example, if your worldview is that people are typically in competition and someone cuts you off on the highway, you may assume that the driver did it purposely to get ahead of you. Other types of beliefs about the way the world is or the way the world should be, for example, political beliefs, can similarly become a significant source of bias.
  • Racism, sexism, ageism and other forms of prejudice and bigotry.  These are, sadly, a common source of bias in many people. They are essentially a special kind of “belief about the way the world is.” These beliefs—for example, that women do not make effective leaders—lead people to ignore contradictory evidence (examples of effective women leaders, or research that disputes the belief) and to interpret ambiguous evidence in a way consistent with the belief.
  • Self-interest.  When particular people benefit from things turning out a certain way, they can sometimes be very susceptible to letting that interest bias them. For example, a company that will earn a profit if they sell their product may have a bias in the way that they give information about their product. A union that will benefit if its members get a generous contract might have a bias in the way it presents information about salaries at competing organizations. (Note that our inclusion of examples describing both companies and unions is an explicit attempt to control for our own personal biases). Home buyers are often dismayed to discover that they purchased their dream house from someone whose self-interest led them to lie about flooding problems in the basement or back yard. This principle, the biasing power of self-interest, is likely what led to the famous phrase  Caveat Emptor  (let the buyer beware) .  

Knowing that these types of biases exist will help you evaluate evidence more critically. Do not forget, though, that people are not always keen to let you discover the sources of biases in their arguments. For example, companies or political organizations can sometimes disguise their support of a research study by contracting with a university professor, who comes complete with a seemingly unbiased institutional affiliation, to conduct the study.

People’s biases, conscious or unconscious, can lead them to make omissions, distortions, and assumptions that undermine our ability to correctly evaluate evidence. It is essential that you look for these elements. Always ask, what is missing, what is not as it appears, and what is being assumed here? For example, consider this (fictional) chart from an ad reporting customer satisfaction at 4 local health clubs.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Clearly, from the results of the chart, one would be tempted to give Club C a try, as customer satisfaction is much higher than for the other 3 clubs.

There are so many distortions and omissions in this chart, however, that it is actually quite meaningless. First, how was satisfaction measured? Do the bars represent responses to a survey? If so, how were the questions asked? Most importantly, where is the missing scale for the chart? Although the differences look quite large, are they really?

Well, here is the same chart, with a different scale, this time labeled:

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Club C is not so impressive any more, is it? In fact, all of the health clubs have customer satisfaction ratings (whatever that means) between 85% and 88%. In the first chart, the entire scale of the graph included only the percentages between 83 and 89. This “judicious” choice of scale—some would call it a distortion—and omission of that scale from the chart make the tiny differences among the clubs seem important, however.

Also, in order to be a critical thinker, you need to learn to pay attention to the assumptions that underlie a message. Let us briefly illustrate the role of assumptions by touching on some people’s beliefs about the criminal justice system in the US. Some believe that a major problem with our judicial system is that many criminals go free because of legal technicalities. Others believe that a major problem is that many innocent people are convicted of crimes. The simple fact is, both types of errors occur. A person’s conclusion about which flaw in our judicial system is the greater tragedy is based on an assumption about which of these is the more serious error (letting the guilty go free or convicting the innocent). This type of assumption is called a value assumption (Browne and Keeley, 2018). It reflects the differences in values that people develop, differences that may lead us to disregard valid evidence that does not fit in with our particular values.

Oh, by the way, some students probably noticed this, but the seven tips for evaluating information that we shared in Module 1 are related to this. Actually, they are part of this section. The tips are, to a very large degree, set of ideas you can use to help you identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions. If you do not remember this section, we strongly recommend you take a few minutes to review it.

skepticism :  a way of thinking in which you refrain from drawing a conclusion or changing your mind until good evidence has been provided

bias : an inclination, tendency, leaning, or prejudice

  • Which of your beliefs (or disbeliefs) from the Activate exercise for this section were derived from a process of critical thinking? If some of your beliefs were not based on critical thinking, are you willing to reassess these beliefs? If the answer is no, why do you think that is? If the answer is yes, what concrete steps will you take?

7.2 Reasoning and Judgment

  • What percentage of kidnappings are committed by strangers?
  • Which area of the house is riskiest: kitchen, bathroom, or stairs?
  • What is the most common cancer in the US?
  • What percentage of workplace homicides are committed by co-workers?

An essential set of procedural thinking skills is  reasoning , the ability to generate and evaluate solid conclusions from a set of statements or evidence. You should note that these conclusions (when they are generated instead of being evaluated) are one key type of inference that we described in Section 7.1. There are two main types of reasoning, deductive and inductive.

Deductive reasoning

Suppose your teacher tells you that if you get an A on the final exam in a course, you will get an A for the whole course. Then, you get an A on the final exam. What will your final course grade be? Most people can see instantly that you can conclude with certainty that you will get an A for the course. This is a type of reasoning called  deductive reasoning , which is defined as reasoning in which a conclusion is guaranteed to be true as long as the statements leading to it are true. The three statements can be listed as an  argument , with two beginning statements and a conclusion:

Statement 1: If you get an A on the final exam, you will get an A for the course

Statement 2: You get an A on the final exam

Conclusion: You will get an A for the course

This particular arrangement, in which true beginning statements lead to a guaranteed true conclusion, is known as a  deductively valid argument . Although deductive reasoning is often the subject of abstract, brain-teasing, puzzle-like word problems, it is actually an extremely important type of everyday reasoning. It is just hard to recognize sometimes. For example, imagine that you are looking for your car keys and you realize that they are either in the kitchen drawer or in your book bag. After looking in the kitchen drawer, you instantly know that they must be in your book bag. That conclusion results from a simple deductive reasoning argument. In addition, solid deductive reasoning skills are necessary for you to succeed in the sciences, philosophy, math, computer programming, and any endeavor involving the use of logic to persuade others to your point of view or to evaluate others’ arguments.

Cognitive psychologists, and before them philosophers, have been quite interested in deductive reasoning, not so much for its practical applications, but for the insights it can offer them about the ways that human beings think. One of the early ideas to emerge from the examination of deductive reasoning is that people learn (or develop) mental versions of rules that allow them to solve these types of reasoning problems (Braine, 1978; Braine, Reiser, & Rumain, 1984). The best way to see this point of view is to realize that there are different possible rules, and some of them are very simple. For example, consider this rule of logic:

therefore q

Logical rules are often presented abstractly, as letters, in order to imply that they can be used in very many specific situations. Here is a concrete version of the of the same rule:

I’ll either have pizza or a hamburger for dinner tonight (p or q)

I won’t have pizza (not p)

Therefore, I’ll have a hamburger (therefore q)

This kind of reasoning seems so natural, so easy, that it is quite plausible that we would use a version of this rule in our daily lives. At least, it seems more plausible than some of the alternative possibilities—for example, that we need to have experience with the specific situation (pizza or hamburger, in this case) in order to solve this type of problem easily. So perhaps there is a form of natural logic (Rips, 1990) that contains very simple versions of logical rules. When we are faced with a reasoning problem that maps onto one of these rules, we use the rule.

But be very careful; things are not always as easy as they seem. Even these simple rules are not so simple. For example, consider the following rule. Many people fail to realize that this rule is just as valid as the pizza or hamburger rule above.

if p, then q

therefore, not p

Concrete version:

If I eat dinner, then I will have dessert

I did not have dessert

Therefore, I did not eat dinner

The simple fact is, it can be very difficult for people to apply rules of deductive logic correctly; as a result, they make many errors when trying to do so. Is this a deductively valid argument or not?

Students who like school study a lot

Students who study a lot get good grades

Jane does not like school

Therefore, Jane does not get good grades

Many people are surprised to discover that this is not a logically valid argument; the conclusion is not guaranteed to be true from the beginning statements. Although the first statement says that students who like school study a lot, it does NOT say that students who do not like school do not study a lot. In other words, it may very well be possible to study a lot without liking school. Even people who sometimes get problems like this right might not be using the rules of deductive reasoning. Instead, they might just be making judgments for examples they know, in this case, remembering instances of people who get good grades despite not liking school.

Making deductive reasoning even more difficult is the fact that there are two important properties that an argument may have. One, it can be valid or invalid (meaning that the conclusion does or does not follow logically from the statements leading up to it). Two, an argument (or more correctly, its conclusion) can be true or false. Here is an example of an argument that is logically valid, but has a false conclusion (at least we think it is false).

Either you are eleven feet tall or the Grand Canyon was created by a spaceship crashing into the earth.

You are not eleven feet tall

Therefore the Grand Canyon was created by a spaceship crashing into the earth

This argument has the exact same form as the pizza or hamburger argument above, making it is deductively valid. The conclusion is so false, however, that it is absurd (of course, the reason the conclusion is false is that the first statement is false). When people are judging arguments, they tend to not observe the difference between deductive validity and the empirical truth of statements or conclusions. If the elements of an argument happen to be true, people are likely to judge the argument logically valid; if the elements are false, they will very likely judge it invalid (Markovits & Bouffard-Bouchard, 1992; Moshman & Franks, 1986). Thus, it seems a stretch to say that people are using these logical rules to judge the validity of arguments. Many psychologists believe that most people actually have very limited deductive reasoning skills (Johnson-Laird, 1999). They argue that when faced with a problem for which deductive logic is required, people resort to some simpler technique, such as matching terms that appear in the statements and the conclusion (Evans, 1982). This might not seem like a problem, but what if reasoners believe that the elements are true and they happen to be wrong; they will would believe that they are using a form of reasoning that guarantees they are correct and yet be wrong.

deductive reasoning :  a type of reasoning in which the conclusion is guaranteed to be true any time the statements leading up to it are true

argument :  a set of statements in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion

deductively valid argument :  an argument for which true beginning statements guarantee that the conclusion is true

Inductive reasoning and judgment

Every day, you make many judgments about the likelihood of one thing or another. Whether you realize it or not, you are practicing  inductive reasoning   on a daily basis. In inductive reasoning arguments, a conclusion is likely whenever the statements preceding it are true. The first thing to notice about inductive reasoning is that, by definition, you can never be sure about your conclusion; you can only estimate how likely the conclusion is. Inductive reasoning may lead you to focus on Memory Encoding and Recoding when you study for the exam, but it is possible the instructor will ask more questions about Memory Retrieval instead. Unlike deductive reasoning, the conclusions you reach through inductive reasoning are only probable, not certain. That is why scientists consider inductive reasoning weaker than deductive reasoning. But imagine how hard it would be for us to function if we could not act unless we were certain about the outcome.

Inductive reasoning can be represented as logical arguments consisting of statements and a conclusion, just as deductive reasoning can be. In an inductive argument, you are given some statements and a conclusion (or you are given some statements and must draw a conclusion). An argument is  inductively strong   if the conclusion would be very probable whenever the statements are true. So, for example, here is an inductively strong argument:

  • Statement #1: The forecaster on Channel 2 said it is going to rain today.
  • Statement #2: The forecaster on Channel 5 said it is going to rain today.
  • Statement #3: It is very cloudy and humid.
  • Statement #4: You just heard thunder.
  • Conclusion (or judgment): It is going to rain today.

Think of the statements as evidence, on the basis of which you will draw a conclusion. So, based on the evidence presented in the four statements, it is very likely that it will rain today. Will it definitely rain today? Certainly not. We can all think of times that the weather forecaster was wrong.

A true story: Some years ago psychology student was watching a baseball playoff game between the St. Louis Cardinals and the Los Angeles Dodgers. A graphic on the screen had just informed the audience that the Cardinal at bat, (Hall of Fame shortstop) Ozzie Smith, a switch hitter batting left-handed for this plate appearance, had never, in nearly 3000 career at-bats, hit a home run left-handed. The student, who had just learned about inductive reasoning in his psychology class, turned to his companion (a Cardinals fan) and smugly said, “It is an inductively strong argument that Ozzie Smith will not hit a home run.” He turned back to face the television just in time to watch the ball sail over the right field fence for a home run. Although the student felt foolish at the time, he was not wrong. It was an inductively strong argument; 3000 at-bats is an awful lot of evidence suggesting that the Wizard of Ozz (as he was known) would not be hitting one out of the park (think of each at-bat without a home run as a statement in an inductive argument). Sadly (for the die-hard Cubs fan and Cardinals-hating student), despite the strength of the argument, the conclusion was wrong.

Given the possibility that we might draw an incorrect conclusion even with an inductively strong argument, we really want to be sure that we do, in fact, make inductively strong arguments. If we judge something probable, it had better be probable. If we judge something nearly impossible, it had better not happen. Think of inductive reasoning, then, as making reasonably accurate judgments of the probability of some conclusion given a set of evidence.

We base many decisions in our lives on inductive reasoning. For example:

Statement #1: Psychology is not my best subject

Statement #2: My psychology instructor has a reputation for giving difficult exams

Statement #3: My first psychology exam was much harder than I expected

Judgment: The next exam will probably be very difficult.

Decision: I will study tonight instead of watching Netflix.

Some other examples of judgments that people commonly make in a school context include judgments of the likelihood that:

  • A particular class will be interesting/useful/difficult
  • You will be able to finish writing a paper by next week if you go out tonight
  • Your laptop’s battery will last through the next trip to the library
  • You will not miss anything important if you skip class tomorrow
  • Your instructor will not notice if you skip class tomorrow
  • You will be able to find a book that you will need for a paper
  • There will be an essay question about Memory Encoding on the next exam

Tversky and Kahneman (1983) recognized that there are two general ways that we might make these judgments; they termed them extensional (i.e., following the laws of probability) and intuitive (i.e., using shortcuts or heuristics, see below). We will use a similar distinction between Type 1 and Type 2 thinking, as described by Keith Stanovich and his colleagues (Evans and Stanovich, 2013; Stanovich and West, 2000). Type 1 thinking is fast, automatic, effortful, and emotional. In fact, it is hardly fair to call it reasoning at all, as judgments just seem to pop into one’s head. Type 2 thinking , on the other hand, is slow, effortful, and logical. So obviously, it is more likely to lead to a correct judgment, or an optimal decision. The problem is, we tend to over-rely on Type 1. Now, we are not saying that Type 2 is the right way to go for every decision or judgment we make. It seems a bit much, for example, to engage in a step-by-step logical reasoning procedure to decide whether we will have chicken or fish for dinner tonight.

Many bad decisions in some very important contexts, however, can be traced back to poor judgments of the likelihood of certain risks or outcomes that result from the use of Type 1 when a more logical reasoning process would have been more appropriate. For example:

Statement #1: It is late at night.

Statement #2: Albert has been drinking beer for the past five hours at a party.

Statement #3: Albert is not exactly sure where he is or how far away home is.

Judgment: Albert will have no difficulty walking home.

Decision: He walks home alone.

As you can see in this example, the three statements backing up the judgment do not really support it. In other words, this argument is not inductively strong because it is based on judgments that ignore the laws of probability. What are the chances that someone facing these conditions will be able to walk home alone easily? And one need not be drunk to make poor decisions based on judgments that just pop into our heads.

The truth is that many of our probability judgments do not come very close to what the laws of probability say they should be. Think about it. In order for us to reason in accordance with these laws, we would need to know the laws of probability, which would allow us to calculate the relationship between particular pieces of evidence and the probability of some outcome (i.e., how much likelihood should change given a piece of evidence), and we would have to do these heavy math calculations in our heads. After all, that is what Type 2 requires. Needless to say, even if we were motivated, we often do not even know how to apply Type 2 reasoning in many cases.

So what do we do when we don’t have the knowledge, skills, or time required to make the correct mathematical judgment? Do we hold off and wait until we can get better evidence? Do we read up on probability and fire up our calculator app so we can compute the correct probability? Of course not. We rely on Type 1 thinking. We “wing it.” That is, we come up with a likelihood estimate using some means at our disposal. Psychologists use the term heuristic to describe the type of “winging it” we are talking about. A  heuristic   is a shortcut strategy that we use to make some judgment or solve some problem (see Section 7.3). Heuristics are easy and quick, think of them as the basic procedures that are characteristic of Type 1.  They can absolutely lead to reasonably good judgments and decisions in some situations (like choosing between chicken and fish for dinner). They are, however, far from foolproof. There are, in fact, quite a lot of situations in which heuristics can lead us to make incorrect judgments, and in many cases the decisions based on those judgments can have serious consequences.

Let us return to the activity that begins this section. You were asked to judge the likelihood (or frequency) of certain events and risks. You were free to come up with your own evidence (or statements) to make these judgments. This is where a heuristic crops up. As a judgment shortcut, we tend to generate specific examples of those very events to help us decide their likelihood or frequency. For example, if we are asked to judge how common, frequent, or likely a particular type of cancer is, many of our statements would be examples of specific cancer cases:

Statement #1: Andy Kaufman (comedian) had lung cancer.

Statement #2: Colin Powell (US Secretary of State) had prostate cancer.

Statement #3: Bob Marley (musician) had skin and brain cancer

Statement #4: Sandra Day O’Connor (Supreme Court Justice) had breast cancer.

Statement #5: Fred Rogers (children’s entertainer) had stomach cancer.

Statement #6: Robin Roberts (news anchor) had breast cancer.

Statement #7: Bette Davis (actress) had breast cancer.

Judgment: Breast cancer is the most common type.

Your own experience or memory may also tell you that breast cancer is the most common type. But it is not (although it is common). Actually, skin cancer is the most common type in the US. We make the same types of misjudgments all the time because we do not generate the examples or evidence according to their actual frequencies or probabilities. Instead, we have a tendency (or bias) to search for the examples in memory; if they are easy to retrieve, we assume that they are common. To rephrase this in the language of the heuristic, events seem more likely to the extent that they are available to memory. This bias has been termed the  availability heuristic   (Kahneman and Tversky, 1974).

The fact that we use the availability heuristic does not automatically mean that our judgment is wrong. The reason we use heuristics in the first place is that they work fairly well in many cases (and, of course that they are easy to use). So, the easiest examples to think of sometimes are the most common ones. Is it more likely that a member of the U.S. Senate is a man or a woman? Most people have a much easier time generating examples of male senators. And as it turns out, the U.S. Senate has many more men than women (74 to 26 in 2020). In this case, then, the availability heuristic would lead you to make the correct judgment; it is far more likely that a senator would be a man.

In many other cases, however, the availability heuristic will lead us astray. This is because events can be memorable for many reasons other than their frequency. Section 5.2, Encoding Meaning, suggested that one good way to encode the meaning of some information is to form a mental image of it. Thus, information that has been pictured mentally will be more available to memory. Indeed, an event that is vivid and easily pictured will trick many people into supposing that type of event is more common than it actually is. Repetition of information will also make it more memorable. So, if the same event is described to you in a magazine, on the evening news, on a podcast that you listen to, and in your Facebook feed; it will be very available to memory. Again, the availability heuristic will cause you to misperceive the frequency of these types of events.

Most interestingly, information that is unusual is more memorable. Suppose we give you the following list of words to remember: box, flower, letter, platypus, oven, boat, newspaper, purse, drum, car. Very likely, the easiest word to remember would be platypus, the unusual one. The same thing occurs with memories of events. An event may be available to memory because it is unusual, yet the availability heuristic leads us to judge that the event is common. Did you catch that? In these cases, the availability heuristic makes us think the exact opposite of the true frequency. We end up thinking something is common because it is unusual (and therefore memorable). Yikes.

The misapplication of the availability heuristic sometimes has unfortunate results. For example, if you went to K-12 school in the US over the past 10 years, it is extremely likely that you have participated in lockdown and active shooter drills. Of course, everyone is trying to prevent the tragedy of another school shooting. And believe us, we are not trying to minimize how terrible the tragedy is. But the truth of the matter is, school shootings are extremely rare. Because the federal government does not keep a database of school shootings, the Washington Post has maintained their own running tally. Between 1999 and January 2020 (the date of the most recent school shooting with a death in the US at of the time this paragraph was written), the Post reported a total of 254 people died in school shootings in the US. Not 254 per year, 254 total. That is an average of 12 per year. Of course, that is 254 people who should not have died (particularly because many were children), but in a country with approximately 60,000,000 students and teachers, this is a very small risk.

But many students and teachers are terrified that they will be victims of school shootings because of the availability heuristic. It is so easy to think of examples (they are very available to memory) that people believe the event is very common. It is not. And there is a downside to this. We happen to believe that there is an enormous gun violence problem in the United States. According the the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, there were 39,773 firearm deaths in the US in 2017. Fifteen of those deaths were in school shootings, according to the Post. 60% of those deaths were suicides. When people pay attention to the school shooting risk (low), they often fail to notice the much larger risk.

And examples like this are by no means unique. The authors of this book have been teaching psychology since the 1990’s. We have been able to make the exact same arguments about the misapplication of the availability heuristics and keep them current by simply swapping out for the “fear of the day.” In the 1990’s it was children being kidnapped by strangers (it was known as “stranger danger”) despite the facts that kidnappings accounted for only 2% of the violent crimes committed against children, and only 24% of kidnappings are committed by strangers (US Department of Justice, 2007). This fear overlapped with the fear of terrorism that gripped the country after the 2001 terrorist attacks on the World Trade Center and US Pentagon and still plagues the population of the US somewhat in 2020. After a well-publicized, sensational act of violence, people are extremely likely to increase their estimates of the chances that they, too, will be victims of terror. Think about the reality, however. In October of 2001, a terrorist mailed anthrax spores to members of the US government and a number of media companies. A total of five people died as a result of this attack. The nation was nearly paralyzed by the fear of dying from the attack; in reality the probability of an individual person dying was 0.00000002.

The availability heuristic can lead you to make incorrect judgments in a school setting as well. For example, suppose you are trying to decide if you should take a class from a particular math professor. You might try to make a judgment of how good a teacher she is by recalling instances of friends and acquaintances making comments about her teaching skill. You may have some examples that suggest that she is a poor teacher very available to memory, so on the basis of the availability heuristic you judge her a poor teacher and decide to take the class from someone else. What if, however, the instances you recalled were all from the same person, and this person happens to be a very colorful storyteller? The subsequent ease of remembering the instances might not indicate that the professor is a poor teacher after all.

Although the availability heuristic is obviously important, it is not the only judgment heuristic we use. Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman examined the role of heuristics in inductive reasoning in a long series of studies. Kahneman received a Nobel Prize in Economics for this research in 2002, and Tversky would have certainly received one as well if he had not died of melanoma at age 59 in 1996 (Nobel Prizes are not awarded posthumously). Kahneman and Tversky demonstrated repeatedly that people do not reason in ways that are consistent with the laws of probability. They identified several heuristic strategies that people use instead to make judgments about likelihood. The importance of this work for economics (and the reason that Kahneman was awarded the Nobel Prize) is that earlier economic theories had assumed that people do make judgments rationally, that is, in agreement with the laws of probability.

Another common heuristic that people use for making judgments is the  representativeness heuristic (Kahneman & Tversky 1973). Suppose we describe a person to you. He is quiet and shy, has an unassuming personality, and likes to work with numbers. Is this person more likely to be an accountant or an attorney? If you said accountant, you were probably using the representativeness heuristic. Our imaginary person is judged likely to be an accountant because he resembles, or is representative of the concept of, an accountant. When research participants are asked to make judgments such as these, the only thing that seems to matter is the representativeness of the description. For example, if told that the person described is in a room that contains 70 attorneys and 30 accountants, participants will still assume that he is an accountant.

inductive reasoning :  a type of reasoning in which we make judgments about likelihood from sets of evidence

inductively strong argument :  an inductive argument in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion that is probably true

heuristic :  a shortcut strategy that we use to make judgments and solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

availability heuristic :  judging the frequency or likelihood of some event type according to how easily examples of the event can be called to mind (i.e., how available they are to memory)

representativeness heuristic:   judging the likelihood that something is a member of a category on the basis of how much it resembles a typical category member (i.e., how representative it is of the category)

Type 1 thinking : fast, automatic, and emotional thinking.

Type 2 thinking : slow, effortful, and logical thinking.

  • What percentage of workplace homicides are co-worker violence?

Many people get these questions wrong. The answers are 10%; stairs; skin; 6%. How close were your answers? Explain how the availability heuristic might have led you to make the incorrect judgments.

  • Can you think of some other judgments that you have made (or beliefs that you have) that might have been influenced by the availability heuristic?

7.3 Problem Solving

  • Please take a few minutes to list a number of problems that you are facing right now.
  • Now write about a problem that you recently solved.
  • What is your definition of a problem?

Mary has a problem. Her daughter, ordinarily quite eager to please, appears to delight in being the last person to do anything. Whether getting ready for school, going to piano lessons or karate class, or even going out with her friends, she seems unwilling or unable to get ready on time. Other people have different kinds of problems. For example, many students work at jobs, have numerous family commitments, and are facing a course schedule full of difficult exams, assignments, papers, and speeches. How can they find enough time to devote to their studies and still fulfill their other obligations? Speaking of students and their problems: Show that a ball thrown vertically upward with initial velocity v0 takes twice as much time to return as to reach the highest point (from Spiegel, 1981).

These are three very different situations, but we have called them all problems. What makes them all the same, despite the differences? A psychologist might define a  problem   as a situation with an initial state, a goal state, and a set of possible intermediate states. Somewhat more meaningfully, we might consider a problem a situation in which you are in here one state (e.g., daughter is always late), you want to be there in another state (e.g., daughter is not always late), and with no obvious way to get from here to there. Defined this way, each of the three situations we outlined can now be seen as an example of the same general concept, a problem. At this point, you might begin to wonder what is not a problem, given such a general definition. It seems that nearly every non-routine task we engage in could qualify as a problem. As long as you realize that problems are not necessarily bad (it can be quite fun and satisfying to rise to the challenge and solve a problem), this may be a useful way to think about it.

Can we identify a set of problem-solving skills that would apply to these very different kinds of situations? That task, in a nutshell, is a major goal of this section. Let us try to begin to make sense of the wide variety of ways that problems can be solved with an important observation: the process of solving problems can be divided into two key parts. First, people have to notice, comprehend, and represent the problem properly in their minds (called  problem representation ). Second, they have to apply some kind of solution strategy to the problem. Psychologists have studied both of these key parts of the process in detail.

When you first think about the problem-solving process, you might guess that most of our difficulties would occur because we are failing in the second step, the application of strategies. Although this can be a significant difficulty much of the time, the more important source of difficulty is probably problem representation. In short, we often fail to solve a problem because we are looking at it, or thinking about it, the wrong way.

problem :  a situation in which we are in an initial state, have a desired goal state, and there is a number of possible intermediate states (i.e., there is no obvious way to get from the initial to the goal state)

problem representation :  noticing, comprehending and forming a mental conception of a problem

Defining and Mentally Representing Problems in Order to Solve Them

So, the main obstacle to solving a problem is that we do not clearly understand exactly what the problem is. Recall the problem with Mary’s daughter always being late. One way to represent, or to think about, this problem is that she is being defiant. She refuses to get ready in time. This type of representation or definition suggests a particular type of solution. Another way to think about the problem, however, is to consider the possibility that she is simply being sidetracked by interesting diversions. This different conception of what the problem is (i.e., different representation) suggests a very different solution strategy. For example, if Mary defines the problem as defiance, she may be tempted to solve the problem using some kind of coercive tactics, that is, to assert her authority as her mother and force her to listen. On the other hand, if Mary defines the problem as distraction, she may try to solve it by simply removing the distracting objects.

As you might guess, when a problem is represented one way, the solution may seem very difficult, or even impossible. Seen another way, the solution might be very easy. For example, consider the following problem (from Nasar, 1998):

Two bicyclists start 20 miles apart and head toward each other, each going at a steady rate of 10 miles per hour. At the same time, a fly that travels at a steady 15 miles per hour starts from the front wheel of the southbound bicycle and flies to the front wheel of the northbound one, then turns around and flies to the front wheel of the southbound one again, and continues in this manner until he is crushed between the two front wheels. Question: what total distance did the fly cover?

Please take a few minutes to try to solve this problem.

Most people represent this problem as a question about a fly because, well, that is how the question is asked. The solution, using this representation, is to figure out how far the fly travels on the first leg of its journey, then add this total to how far it travels on the second leg of its journey (when it turns around and returns to the first bicycle), then continue to add the smaller distance from each leg of the journey until you converge on the correct answer. You would have to be quite skilled at math to solve this problem, and you would probably need some time and pencil and paper to do it.

If you consider a different representation, however, you can solve this problem in your head. Instead of thinking about it as a question about a fly, think about it as a question about the bicycles. They are 20 miles apart, and each is traveling 10 miles per hour. How long will it take for the bicycles to reach each other? Right, one hour. The fly is traveling 15 miles per hour; therefore, it will travel a total of 15 miles back and forth in the hour before the bicycles meet. Represented one way (as a problem about a fly), the problem is quite difficult. Represented another way (as a problem about two bicycles), it is easy. Changing your representation of a problem is sometimes the best—sometimes the only—way to solve it.

Unfortunately, however, changing a problem’s representation is not the easiest thing in the world to do. Often, problem solvers get stuck looking at a problem one way. This is called  fixation . Most people who represent the preceding problem as a problem about a fly probably do not pause to reconsider, and consequently change, their representation. A parent who thinks her daughter is being defiant is unlikely to consider the possibility that her behavior is far less purposeful.

Problem-solving fixation was examined by a group of German psychologists called Gestalt psychologists during the 1930’s and 1940’s. Karl Dunker, for example, discovered an important type of failure to take a different perspective called  functional fixedness . Imagine being a participant in one of his experiments. You are asked to figure out how to mount two candles on a door and are given an assortment of odds and ends, including a small empty cardboard box and some thumbtacks. Perhaps you have already figured out a solution: tack the box to the door so it forms a platform, then put the candles on top of the box. Most people are able to arrive at this solution. Imagine a slight variation of the procedure, however. What if, instead of being empty, the box had matches in it? Most people given this version of the problem do not arrive at the solution given above. Why? Because it seems to people that when the box contains matches, it already has a function; it is a matchbox. People are unlikely to consider a new function for an object that already has a function. This is functional fixedness.

Mental set is a type of fixation in which the problem solver gets stuck using the same solution strategy that has been successful in the past, even though the solution may no longer be useful. It is commonly seen when students do math problems for homework. Often, several problems in a row require the reapplication of the same solution strategy. Then, without warning, the next problem in the set requires a new strategy. Many students attempt to apply the formerly successful strategy on the new problem and therefore cannot come up with a correct answer.

The thing to remember is that you cannot solve a problem unless you correctly identify what it is to begin with (initial state) and what you want the end result to be (goal state). That may mean looking at the problem from a different angle and representing it in a new way. The correct representation does not guarantee a successful solution, but it certainly puts you on the right track.

A bit more optimistically, the Gestalt psychologists discovered what may be considered the opposite of fixation, namely  insight . Sometimes the solution to a problem just seems to pop into your head. Wolfgang Kohler examined insight by posing many different problems to chimpanzees, principally problems pertaining to their acquisition of out-of-reach food. In one version, a banana was placed outside of a chimpanzee’s cage and a short stick inside the cage. The stick was too short to retrieve the banana, but was long enough to retrieve a longer stick also located outside of the cage. This second stick was long enough to retrieve the banana. After trying, and failing, to reach the banana with the shorter stick, the chimpanzee would try a couple of random-seeming attempts, react with some apparent frustration or anger, then suddenly rush to the longer stick, the correct solution fully realized at this point. This sudden appearance of the solution, observed many times with many different problems, was termed insight by Kohler.

Lest you think it pertains to chimpanzees only, Karl Dunker demonstrated that children also solve problems through insight in the 1930s. More importantly, you have probably experienced insight yourself. Think back to a time when you were trying to solve a difficult problem. After struggling for a while, you gave up. Hours later, the solution just popped into your head, perhaps when you were taking a walk, eating dinner, or lying in bed.

fixation :  when a problem solver gets stuck looking at a problem a particular way and cannot change his or her representation of it (or his or her intended solution strategy)

functional fixedness :  a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver cannot think of a new use for an object that already has a function

mental set :  a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver gets stuck using the same solution strategy that has been successful in the past

insight :  a sudden realization of a solution to a problem

Solving Problems by Trial and Error

Correctly identifying the problem and your goal for a solution is a good start, but recall the psychologist’s definition of a problem: it includes a set of possible intermediate states. Viewed this way, a problem can be solved satisfactorily only if one can find a path through some of these intermediate states to the goal. Imagine a fairly routine problem, finding a new route to school when your ordinary route is blocked (by road construction, for example). At each intersection, you may turn left, turn right, or go straight. A satisfactory solution to the problem (of getting to school) is a sequence of selections at each intersection that allows you to wind up at school.

If you had all the time in the world to get to school, you might try choosing intermediate states randomly. At one corner you turn left, the next you go straight, then you go left again, then right, then right, then straight. Unfortunately, trial and error will not necessarily get you where you want to go, and even if it does, it is not the fastest way to get there. For example, when a friend of ours was in college, he got lost on the way to a concert and attempted to find the venue by choosing streets to turn onto randomly (this was long before the use of GPS). Amazingly enough, the strategy worked, although he did end up missing two out of the three bands who played that night.

Trial and error is not all bad, however. B.F. Skinner, a prominent behaviorist psychologist, suggested that people often behave randomly in order to see what effect the behavior has on the environment and what subsequent effect this environmental change has on them. This seems particularly true for the very young person. Picture a child filling a household’s fish tank with toilet paper, for example. To a child trying to develop a repertoire of creative problem-solving strategies, an odd and random behavior might be just the ticket. Eventually, the exasperated parent hopes, the child will discover that many of these random behaviors do not successfully solve problems; in fact, in many cases they create problems. Thus, one would expect a decrease in this random behavior as a child matures. You should realize, however, that the opposite extreme is equally counterproductive. If the children become too rigid, never trying something unexpected and new, their problem solving skills can become too limited.

Effective problem solving seems to call for a happy medium that strikes a balance between using well-founded old strategies and trying new ground and territory. The individual who recognizes a situation in which an old problem-solving strategy would work best, and who can also recognize a situation in which a new untested strategy is necessary is halfway to success.

Solving Problems with Algorithms and Heuristics

For many problems there is a possible strategy available that will guarantee a correct solution. For example, think about math problems. Math lessons often consist of step-by-step procedures that can be used to solve the problems. If you apply the strategy without error, you are guaranteed to arrive at the correct solution to the problem. This approach is called using an  algorithm , a term that denotes the step-by-step procedure that guarantees a correct solution. Because algorithms are sometimes available and come with a guarantee, you might think that most people use them frequently. Unfortunately, however, they do not. As the experience of many students who have struggled through math classes can attest, algorithms can be extremely difficult to use, even when the problem solver knows which algorithm is supposed to work in solving the problem. In problems outside of math class, we often do not even know if an algorithm is available. It is probably fair to say, then, that algorithms are rarely used when people try to solve problems.

Because algorithms are so difficult to use, people often pass up the opportunity to guarantee a correct solution in favor of a strategy that is much easier to use and yields a reasonable chance of coming up with a correct solution. These strategies are called  problem solving heuristics . Similar to what you saw in section 6.2 with reasoning heuristics, a problem solving heuristic is a shortcut strategy that people use when trying to solve problems. It usually works pretty well, but does not guarantee a correct solution to the problem. For example, one problem solving heuristic might be “always move toward the goal” (so when trying to get to school when your regular route is blocked, you would always turn in the direction you think the school is). A heuristic that people might use when doing math homework is “use the same solution strategy that you just used for the previous problem.”

By the way, we hope these last two paragraphs feel familiar to you. They seem to parallel a distinction that you recently learned. Indeed, algorithms and problem-solving heuristics are another example of the distinction between Type 1 thinking and Type 2 thinking.

Although it is probably not worth describing a large number of specific heuristics, two observations about heuristics are worth mentioning. First, heuristics can be very general or they can be very specific, pertaining to a particular type of problem only. For example, “always move toward the goal” is a general strategy that you can apply to countless problem situations. On the other hand, “when you are lost without a functioning gps, pick the most expensive car you can see and follow it” is specific to the problem of being lost. Second, all heuristics are not equally useful. One heuristic that many students know is “when in doubt, choose c for a question on a multiple-choice exam.” This is a dreadful strategy because many instructors intentionally randomize the order of answer choices. Another test-taking heuristic, somewhat more useful, is “look for the answer to one question somewhere else on the exam.”

You really should pay attention to the application of heuristics to test taking. Imagine that while reviewing your answers for a multiple-choice exam before turning it in, you come across a question for which you originally thought the answer was c. Upon reflection, you now think that the answer might be b. Should you change the answer to b, or should you stick with your first impression? Most people will apply the heuristic strategy to “stick with your first impression.” What they do not realize, of course, is that this is a very poor strategy (Lilienfeld et al, 2009). Most of the errors on exams come on questions that were answered wrong originally and were not changed (so they remain wrong). There are many fewer errors where we change a correct answer to an incorrect answer. And, of course, sometimes we change an incorrect answer to a correct answer. In fact, research has shown that it is more common to change a wrong answer to a right answer than vice versa (Bruno, 2001).

The belief in this poor test-taking strategy (stick with your first impression) is based on the  confirmation bias   (Nickerson, 1998; Wason, 1960). You first saw the confirmation bias in Module 1, but because it is so important, we will repeat the information here. People have a bias, or tendency, to notice information that confirms what they already believe. Somebody at one time told you to stick with your first impression, so when you look at the results of an exam you have taken, you will tend to notice the cases that are consistent with that belief. That is, you will notice the cases in which you originally had an answer correct and changed it to the wrong answer. You tend not to notice the other two important (and more common) cases, changing an answer from wrong to right, and leaving a wrong answer unchanged.

Because heuristics by definition do not guarantee a correct solution to a problem, mistakes are bound to occur when we employ them. A poor choice of a specific heuristic will lead to an even higher likelihood of making an error.

algorithm :  a step-by-step procedure that guarantees a correct solution to a problem

problem solving heuristic :  a shortcut strategy that we use to solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

confirmation bias :  people’s tendency to notice information that confirms what they already believe

An Effective Problem-Solving Sequence

You may be left with a big question: If algorithms are hard to use and heuristics often don’t work, how am I supposed to solve problems? Robert Sternberg (1996), as part of his theory of what makes people successfully intelligent (Module 8) described a problem-solving sequence that has been shown to work rather well:

  • Identify the existence of a problem.  In school, problem identification is often easy; problems that you encounter in math classes, for example, are conveniently labeled as problems for you. Outside of school, however, realizing that you have a problem is a key difficulty that you must get past in order to begin solving it. You must be very sensitive to the symptoms that indicate a problem.
  • Define the problem.  Suppose you realize that you have been having many headaches recently. Very likely, you would identify this as a problem. If you define the problem as “headaches,” the solution would probably be to take aspirin or ibuprofen or some other anti-inflammatory medication. If the headaches keep returning, however, you have not really solved the problem—likely because you have mistaken a symptom for the problem itself. Instead, you must find the root cause of the headaches. Stress might be the real problem. For you to successfully solve many problems it may be necessary for you to overcome your fixations and represent the problems differently. One specific strategy that you might find useful is to try to define the problem from someone else’s perspective. How would your parents, spouse, significant other, doctor, etc. define the problem? Somewhere in these different perspectives may lurk the key definition that will allow you to find an easier and permanent solution.
  • Formulate strategy.  Now it is time to begin planning exactly how the problem will be solved. Is there an algorithm or heuristic available for you to use? Remember, heuristics by their very nature guarantee that occasionally you will not be able to solve the problem. One point to keep in mind is that you should look for long-range solutions, which are more likely to address the root cause of a problem than short-range solutions.
  • Represent and organize information.  Similar to the way that the problem itself can be defined, or represented in multiple ways, information within the problem is open to different interpretations. Suppose you are studying for a big exam. You have chapters from a textbook and from a supplemental reader, along with lecture notes that all need to be studied. How should you (represent and) organize these materials? Should you separate them by type of material (text versus reader versus lecture notes), or should you separate them by topic? To solve problems effectively, you must learn to find the most useful representation and organization of information.
  • Allocate resources.  This is perhaps the simplest principle of the problem solving sequence, but it is extremely difficult for many people. First, you must decide whether time, money, skills, effort, goodwill, or some other resource would help to solve the problem Then, you must make the hard choice of deciding which resources to use, realizing that you cannot devote maximum resources to every problem. Very often, the solution to problem is simply to change how resources are allocated (for example, spending more time studying in order to improve grades).
  • Monitor and evaluate solutions.  Pay attention to the solution strategy while you are applying it. If it is not working, you may be able to select another strategy. Another fact you should realize about problem solving is that it never does end. Solving one problem frequently brings up new ones. Good monitoring and evaluation of your problem solutions can help you to anticipate and get a jump on solving the inevitable new problems that will arise.

Please note that this as  an  effective problem-solving sequence, not  the  effective problem solving sequence. Just as you can become fixated and end up representing the problem incorrectly or trying an inefficient solution, you can become stuck applying the problem-solving sequence in an inflexible way. Clearly there are problem situations that can be solved without using these skills in this order.

Additionally, many real-world problems may require that you go back and redefine a problem several times as the situation changes (Sternberg et al. 2000). For example, consider the problem with Mary’s daughter one last time. At first, Mary did represent the problem as one of defiance. When her early strategy of pleading and threatening punishment was unsuccessful, Mary began to observe her daughter more carefully. She noticed that, indeed, her daughter’s attention would be drawn by an irresistible distraction or book. Fresh with a re-representation of the problem, she began a new solution strategy. She began to remind her daughter every few minutes to stay on task and remind her that if she is ready before it is time to leave, she may return to the book or other distracting object at that time. Fortunately, this strategy was successful, so Mary did not have to go back and redefine the problem again.

Pick one or two of the problems that you listed when you first started studying this section and try to work out the steps of Sternberg’s problem solving sequence for each one.

a mental representation of a category of things in the world

an assumption about the truth of something that is not stated. Inferences come from our prior knowledge and experience, and from logical reasoning

knowledge about one’s own cognitive processes; thinking about your thinking

individuals who are less competent tend to overestimate their abilities more than individuals who are more competent do

Thinking like a scientist in your everyday life for the purpose of drawing correct conclusions. It entails skepticism; an ability to identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions; and excellent deductive and inductive reasoning, and problem solving skills.

a way of thinking in which you refrain from drawing a conclusion or changing your mind until good evidence has been provided

an inclination, tendency, leaning, or prejudice

a type of reasoning in which the conclusion is guaranteed to be true any time the statements leading up to it are true

a set of statements in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion

an argument for which true beginning statements guarantee that the conclusion is true

a type of reasoning in which we make judgments about likelihood from sets of evidence

an inductive argument in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion that is probably true

fast, automatic, and emotional thinking

slow, effortful, and logical thinking

a shortcut strategy that we use to make judgments and solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

udging the frequency or likelihood of some event type according to how easily examples of the event can be called to mind (i.e., how available they are to memory)

judging the likelihood that something is a member of a category on the basis of how much it resembles a typical category member (i.e., how representative it is of the category)

a situation in which we are in an initial state, have a desired goal state, and there is a number of possible intermediate states (i.e., there is no obvious way to get from the initial to the goal state)

noticing, comprehending and forming a mental conception of a problem

when a problem solver gets stuck looking at a problem a particular way and cannot change his or her representation of it (or his or her intended solution strategy)

a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver cannot think of a new use for an object that already has a function

a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver gets stuck using the same solution strategy that has been successful in the past

a sudden realization of a solution to a problem

a step-by-step procedure that guarantees a correct solution to a problem

The tendency to notice and pay attention to information that confirms your prior beliefs and to ignore information that disconfirms them.

a shortcut strategy that we use to solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

Introduction to Psychology Copyright © 2020 by Ken Gray; Elizabeth Arnott-Hill; and Or'Shaundra Benson is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Logo for University of Central Florida Pressbooks

Thinking and Intelligence

Introduction to Thinking and Problem-Solving

What you’ll learn to do: describe cognition and problem-solving strategies.

A man sitting down in "The Thinker" pose.

Imagine all of your thoughts as if they were physical entities, swirling rapidly inside your mind. How is it possible that the brain is able to move from one thought to the next in an organized, orderly fashion? The brain is endlessly perceiving, processing, planning, organizing, and remembering—it is always active. Yet, you don’t notice most of your brain’s activity as you move throughout your daily routine. This is only one facet of the complex processes involved in cognition. Simply put, cognition is thinking, and it encompasses the processes associated with perception, knowledge, problem solving, judgment, language, and memory. Scientists who study cognition are searching for ways to understand how we integrate, organize, and utilize our conscious cognitive experiences without being aware of all of the unconscious work that our brains are doing (for example, Kahneman, 2011).

Learning Objectives

  • Distinguish between concepts and prototypes
  • Explain the difference between natural and artificial concepts
  • Describe problem solving strategies, including algorithms and heuristics
  • Explain some common roadblocks to effective problem solving

CC licensed content, Original

  • Modification, adaptation, and original content. Provided by : Lumen Learning. License : CC BY: Attribution

CC licensed content, Shared previously

  • What Is Cognition?. Authored by : OpenStax College. Located at : https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/7-1-what-is-cognition . License : CC BY: Attribution . License Terms : Download for free at https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/1-introduction
  • A Thinking Man Image. Authored by : Wesley Nitsckie. Located at : https://www.flickr.com/photos/nitsckie/5507777269 . License : CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike

General Psychology Copyright © by OpenStax and Lumen Learning is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Library homepage

  • school Campus Bookshelves
  • menu_book Bookshelves
  • perm_media Learning Objects
  • login Login
  • how_to_reg Request Instructor Account
  • hub Instructor Commons
  • Download Page (PDF)
  • Download Full Book (PDF)
  • Periodic Table
  • Physics Constants
  • Scientific Calculator
  • Reference & Cite
  • Tools expand_more
  • Readability

selected template will load here

This action is not available.

Social Sci LibreTexts

11.4: Information Processing- Learning, Memory, and Problem Solving

  • Last updated
  • Save as PDF
  • Page ID 24662

  • Paris, Ricardo, Raymond, & Johnson
  • College of the Canyons via College of the Canyons

During middle and late childhood children make strides in several areas of cognitive function including the capacity of working memory, their ability to pay attention, and their use of memory strategies. Both changes in the brain and experience foster these abilities. In this section, we will look at how children process information, think and learn, allowing them to increase their ability to learn and remember due to an improvement in the ways they attend to, store information, and problem solve.29

Working Memory: The capacity of working memory expands during middle and late childhood, research has suggested that both an increase in processing speed and the ability to inhibit irrelevant information from entering memory are contributing to the greater efficiency of working memory during this age (de Ribaupierre, 2002). Changes in myelination and synaptic pruning in the cortex are likely behind the increase in processing speed and ability to filter out irrelevant stimuli (Kail, McBride-Chang, Ferrer, Cho, & Shu, 2013).

clipboard_e7a4a9bc0aa507d1ff6fc19cd9c4533e3.png

Attention: As noted above, the ability to inhibit irrelevant information improves during this age group, with there being a sharp improvement in selective attention from age six into adolescence (Vakil, Blachstein, Sheinman, & Greenstein, 2009). Children also improve in their ability to shift their attention between tasks or different features of a task (Carlson, Zelazo, & Faja, 2013). A younger child who is asked to sort objects into piles based on the type of object, car versus animal, or color of the object, red versus blue, would likely have no trouble doing so. But if you ask them to switch from sorting based on type to now having them sort based on color, they would struggle because this requires them to suppress the prior sorting rule. An older child has less difficulty making the switch, meaning there is greater flexibility in their intentional skills. These changes in attention and working memory contribute to children having more strategic approaches to challenging tasks.

Memory Strategies: Bjorklund (2005) describes a developmental progression in the acquisition and use of memory strategies. Such strategies are often lacking in younger children, but increase in frequency as children progress through elementary school. Examples of memory strategies include rehearsing information you wish to recall, visualizing and organizing information, creating rhymes, such as “i” before “e” except after “c”, or inventing acronyms, such as “roygbiv” to remember the colors of the rainbow. Schneider, Kron-Sperl, and Hünnerkopf (2009) reported a steady increase in the use of memory strategies from ages six to ten in their longitudinal study. Moreover, by age ten many children were using two or more memory strategies to help them recall information. Schneider and colleagues found that there were considerable individual differences at each age in the use of strategies and that children who utilized more strategies had better memory performance than their same-aged peers.

Contributors and Attributions

29. Lifespan Development: A Psychological Perspective by Martha Lally and Suzanne Valentine-French is licensed under CC BY-NC-SA 3.0

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2023 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Cognition in Psychology

How People Think and What's Involved in This Process

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Daniel B. Block, MD, is an award-winning, board-certified psychiatrist who operates a private practice in Pennsylvania.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Verywell / Laura Porter

Definition of Cognition

  • Improvement Tips

Frequently Asked Questions

Cognition is a term referring to the mental processes involved in gaining knowledge and comprehension. Some of the many different cognitive processes include thinking, knowing, remembering, judging, and problem-solving .

These are higher-level functions of the brain and encompass language, imagination, perception, and planning. Cognitive psychology is the field of psychology that investigates how people think and the processes involved in cognition. 

What is an example of cognition?

Cognition includes all of the conscious and unconscious processes involved in thinking, perceiving, and reasoning. Examples of cognition include paying attention to something in the environment, learning something new, making decisions, processing language, sensing and perceiving environmental stimuli, solving problems, and using memory. 

History of the Study of Cognition

The study of how humans think dates back to the time of ancient Greek philosophers Plato and Aristotle.

Philosophical Origins

Plato's approach to the study of the mind suggested that people understand the world by first identifying basic principles buried deep inside themselves, then using rational thought to create knowledge. This viewpoint was later advocated by philosophers such as Rene Descartes and linguist Noam Chomsky. It is often referred to as rationalism.

Aristotle, on the other hand, believed that people acquire knowledge through their observations of the world around them. Later thinkers such as John Locke and B.F. Skinner also advocated this point of view, which is often referred to as empiricism.

Early Psychology

During the earliest days of psychology—and for the first half of the 20th century—psychology was largely dominated by psychoanalysis , behaviorism , and humanism .

Eventually, a formal field of study devoted solely to the study of cognition emerged as part of the "cognitive revolution" of the 1960s. This field is known as cognitive psychology.

The Emergence of Cognitive Psychology

One of the earliest definitions of cognition was presented in the first textbook on cognitive psychology, which was published in 1967. According to Ulric Neisser, a psychologist and the book's author, cognition is "those processes by which the sensory input is transformed, reduced, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used."

Types of Cognitive Processes

There are many different types of cognitive processes. They include:

  • Attention : Attention is a cognitive process that allows people to focus on a specific stimulus in the environment.
  • Language : Language and language development are cognitive processes that involve the ability to understand and express thoughts through spoken and written words. This allows us to communicate with others and plays an important role in thought.
  • Learning : Learning requires cognitive processes involved in taking in new things, synthesizing information, and integrating it with prior knowledge.
  • Memory : Memory is an important cognitive process that allows people to encode, store, and retrieve information. It is a critical component in the learning process and allows people to retain knowledge about the world and their personal histories.
  • Perception : Perception is a cognitive process that allows people to take in information through their senses, then utilize this information to respond and interact with the world.
  • Thought : Thought is an essential part of every cognitive process. It allows people to engage in decision-making , problem-solving, and higher reasoning.

Hot Cognition vs. Cold Cognition

Some split cognition into two categories: hot and cold. Hot cognition refers to mental processes in which emotion plays a role, such as reward-based learning . Conversely, cold cognition refers to mental processes that don't involve feelings or emotions, such as working memory .

What Can Affect Cognition?

It is important to remember that these cognitive processes are complex and often imperfect. Some of the factors that can affect or influence cognition include:

Research indicates that as we age, our cognitive function tends to decline. Age-related cognitive changes include processing things more slowly, finding it harder to recall past events, and a failure to remember information that was once known (such as how to solve a particular math equation or historical information).

Attention Issues

Selective attention is a limited resource, so there are a number of things that can make it difficult to focus on everything in your environment. Attentional blink , for example, happens when you are so focused on one thing that you completely miss something else happening right in front of you.

Cognitive Biases

Cognitive biases are systematic errors in thinking related to how people process and interpret information about the world. Confirmation bias is one common example that involves only paying attention to information that aligns with your existing beliefs while ignoring evidence that doesn't support your views. 

Some studies have connected cognitive function with certain genes. For example, a 2020 study published in Brain Communications found that a person's level of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which is 30% determined by heritability, can impact the rate of brain neurodegeneration, a condition that ultimately impacts cognitive function.

Memory Limitations

Short-term memory is surprisingly brief, typically lasting just 20 to 30 seconds, whereas long-term memory can be stable and enduring, with memories lasting years and even decades. Memory can also be fragile and fallible. Sometimes we forget and other times we are subject to misinformation effects that may even lead to the formation of false memories .

Uses of Cognition

Cognitive processes affect every aspect of life, from school to work to relationships. Some specific uses for these processes include the following.

Learning New Things

Learning requires being able to take in new information, form new memories, and make connections with other things that you already know. Researchers and educators use their knowledge of these cognitive processes to create instructive materials to help people learn new concepts .

Forming Memories

Memory is a major topic of interest in the field of cognitive psychology. How we remember, what we remember, and what we forget reveal a great deal about how cognitive processes operate.

While people often think of memory as being much like a video camera—carefully recording, cataloging, and storing life events away for later recall—research has found that memory is much more complex.

Making Decisions

Whenever people make any type of a decision, it involves making judgments about things they have processed. This might involve comparing new information to prior knowledge, integrating new information into existing ideas, or even replacing old knowledge with new knowledge before making a choice.

Impact of Cognition

Our cognitive processes have a wide-ranging impact that influences everything from our daily life to our overall health.

Perceiving the World

As you take in sensations from the world around you, the information that you see, hear, taste, touch, and smell must first be transformed into signals that the brain can understand. The perceptual process allows you to take in this sensory information and convert it into a signal that your brain can recognize and act upon.

Forming Impressions

The world is full of an endless number of sensory experiences . To make meaning out of all this incoming information, it is important for the brain to be able to capture the fundamentals. Events are reduced to only the critical concepts and ideas that we need.

Filling in the Gaps

In addition to reducing information to make it more memorable and understandable, people also elaborate on these memories as they reconstruct them. In some cases, this elaboration happens when people are struggling to remember something . When the information cannot be recalled, the brain sometimes fills in the missing data with whatever seems to fit.

Interacting With the World

Cognition involves not only the things that go on inside our heads but also how these thoughts and mental processes influence our actions. Our attention to the world around us, memories of past events, understanding of language, judgments about how the world works, and abilities to solve problems all contribute to how we behave and interact with our surrounding environment.

Tips for Improving Cognition

Cognitive processes are influenced by a range of factors, including genetics and experiences. While you cannot change your genes or age, there are things that you can do to protect and maximize your cognitive abilities:

  • Stay healthy . Lifestyle factors such as eating a nutritious diet and getting regular exercise can have a positive effect on cognitive functioning.  
  • Think critically . Question your assumptions and ask questions about your thoughts, beliefs, and conclusions.
  • Stay curious and keep learning . A great way to flex your cognitive abilities is to keep challenging yourself to learn more about the world.
  • Skip multitasking . While it might seem like doing several things at once would help you get done faster, research has shown it actually decreases both productivity and work quality.

Thinking is an important component, but cognition also encompasses unconscious and perceptual processes as well. In addition to thinking, cognition involves language, attention, learning, memory, and perception.

People utilize cognitive skills to think, learn, recall, and reason. Five important cognitive skills include short-term memory, logic, processing speed, attention, and spatial recognition.

American Psychological Association. Cognition .

Ezebuilo HC. Descartes, Leibniz and Spinoza: A brief survey of rationalism . J App Philos . 2020;18(6):95-118. doi:10.13140/RG.2.2.19692.39043

Sgarbi M.  The Aristotelian Tradition and the Rise of British Empiricism: Logic and Epistemology in the British Isles (1570–1689) .

Lachman R, Lachman J L, Butterfield EC.  Cognitive psychology and information processing: An introduction .

Neisser U.  Cognitive psychology: Classic edition .

Murman D. The impact of age on cognition . Semin Hear . 2015;36(3):111-121. doi:10.1055/s-0035-1555115

Li S, Weinstein G, Zare H, et al. The genetics of circulating BDNF: Towards understanding the role of BDNF in brain structure and function in middle and old ages . Brain Commun . 2020;2(2):fcaa176. doi:10.1093/braincomms/fcaa176

Weinsten Y. How long is short-term memory: Shorter than you might think . Duke Undergraduate Education.

Leding J, Antonio L. Need for cognition and discrepancy detection in the misinformation effect . J Cognitive Psychol . 2019;31(4):409-415. doi:10.1080/20445911.2019.1626400

Scheiter K, Schubert C, Schuler A. Self-regulated learning from illustrated text: Eye movement modelling to support use and regulation of cognitive processes during learning from multimedia . Brit J Educ Psychol . 2017;88(1):80-94. doi:10.1111/bjep.12175

Toppi J, Astolfi L, Risetti M, et al. Different topological properties of EEG-derived networks describe working memory phases as revealed by graph theoretical analysis . Front Hum Neurosci . 2018;11:637. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00637

Mather G. Foundations of sensation and perception .

Sousa D.  How the brain learns .

Houben S, Otgaar H, Roelofs J, Merckelbach H. EMDR and false memories: A response to Lee, de Jongh, and Hase (2019) . Clin Psycholog Sci . 2019;7(3):405-6. doi:10.1177/2167702619830392

Schwarzer R. Self-efficacy: Thought control of action .

Imaoka M, Nakao H, Nakamura M, et al. Effect of multicomponent exercise and nutrition support on the cognitive function of older adults: A randomized controlled trial . Clin Interv Aging . 2019;14:2145-53. doi:10.2147/CIA.S229034

Petroutsatou K, Sifiniadis A. Exploring the consequences of human multitasking in industrial automation projects: A tool to mitigate impacts - Part II . Organiz Techn Manage Construct . 2018;10(1):1770-1777. doi:10.2478/otmcj-2016-0031

Mullis CE, Hatfield RC. The effects of multitasking on auditors' judgment quality . Contemp Account Res . 2017;35(1):314-333. doi:10.1111/1911-3846.12392

Revlin R. Cognition: Theory and Practice .

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

PowerShow.com - The best place to view and share online presentations

  • Preferences

Free template

Cognitive Processes: Memory and Problem Solving - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Cognitive Processes: Memory and Problem Solving

Cognitive processes: memory and problem solving chapters 8 and 9 – powerpoint ppt presentation.

  • Chapters 8 and 9
  • Defining Memory
  • The persistence of learning over time through storage and retrieval of information
  • Much of what we have learned about memory comes from cases of memory loss or extraordinary memory
  • Russian journalist Shereshevskii (case reading)
  • Today, memory is often seen as steps in an information-processing model
  • Information Processing is similar to how computer works
  • Initial recording of information as fleeting sensory memory (1)
  • Processing of this information in short-term memory (STM), where we encode it through rehearsal (2)
  • Also known as working memory
  • We process what we pay attention to of all the incoming sensory stimuli we are bombarded with
  • Working memory also associates new information with old information from LTM
  • Moving of information into more permanent long-term memory (LTM) for later retrieval (3)
  • Memory ability varies from person to person
  • Those with better working memories tend to exhibit high intelligence
  • Those with better memories tend to maintain better focus on tasks
  • Because the brain engages in parallel processing, it automatically gathers information about many things without us even knowing!
  • Space automatically recalling where a term was on a slide
  • Time automatically recalling the sequence of events
  • Frequency automatically keeping track of how many times you have done something
  • Well-learned information automatic processing of very familiar information like words we know
  • New tasks that may begin as effortful can become automatic if we expose ourselves enough!
  • DO NOT write down the following list. Listen and read them, then try to recall them when prompted
  • While some information is automatically and effortlessly processed, some requires focus and attention
  • Attention is defined as selection certain information for further processing
  • We normally pay attention to only a small portion of incoming information
  • We pay attention to things according to
  • Meaningfulness
  • Distinctiveness
  • Broadbents Cocktail Party Phenomenon we tune out other sounds to focus on what is important
  • Can increase memory ability through rehearsal
  • Hermann Ebbinghaus scientifically studied his own memory of verbal information in the 19th century.
  • Memorized a list of nonsense syllables
  • The more he rehearsed, the more he remembered
  • The more time spent on learning, the more we remember
  • One of the important memory phenomena discovered by Ebbinghaus is the overlearning effect.
  • continue to practice memorizing a list beyond that required to produce two perfect recalls.
  • For example, if it required 10 repetitions to memorize the list, then you might continue for an additional ten repetitions -- this would be "100 overlearning."
  • The effect of overlearning is to make the information more resistant to disruption or loss.
  • Information quickly learned is information quickly lost!
  • Spacing Effect distributed study time yields better long-term retention than massed practice (cramming)!
  • Testing Effect more frequent quizzing or previously studied material yields better long-term retention
  • Bottom line spread your studying out over time and quiz yourself frequently!
  • When we are given a list of things to remember, we often remember the first items and last items on the list.
  • This is called the serial position effect.
  • We are able to rehearse those at the beginning the most and those at the end are still in our working memory!
  • Primacy Effect tendency to recall the first items on the list
  • Recency Effect tendency to recall the last items on the list
  • When we hear the sounds that comprise words, we use context and experience to determine meaning.
  • E.g. Eye-Screem is it ice cream or I scream?
  • We tend not to remember literally what we read or heard, but rather the general meaning
  • We use several types of encoding to process verbal information
  • Visual (structural) encoding of images
  • Acoustic (phonetic) encoding of sounds
  • Semantic encoding of meaning
  • Our brains process each type differently
  • Levels of Processing Theory says deeper levels of processing result in longer-lasting memories (i.e. semantic memory is best)
  • Paivios Dual Code Theory says that we remember best when we use both semantic and visual encoding.
  • Which type of encoding visual, acoustic or semantic helps us to remember best?
  • Craik and Tulving (1975)
  • Subjects forced to encode words in the three ways
  • Visually (is it capitalized?)
  • Acoustically (does it rhyme with)
  • semantically (Would it fit into the sentence?)
  • Found that semantic encoding yielded best recall!
  • Spend TIME learning and make MEANING out of what you wish to recall!
  • Self-Reference Effect meaning that is personal relate information to ourselves.
  • If you can recall information, it must first be stored!
  • According to Atkinson and Shiffrin 3-step model, the first stop is sensory memory or the sensory registers.
  • Only holds an exact copy of sensory stimulus for a few fractions of a second.
  • More information enters our sensory memory than will get to STM
  • Sperlings partial report technique illustrates the limitation of sensory memory.
  • When subjects were told to report ALL 9 letters flashed for a fraction of a second, they could not, but when subjects instructed to report 3 of the letters (one row of 3 letter row display of 9 total letters) they could do so with few errors
  • This proved that sensory memory could hold the information, but only very briefly
  • Sensory memory holds information just long enough to recognize and transfer it to STM for further processing
  • This happens through selective attention
  • Selective attention allows only a small percentage of sensory messages that bombard us to enter conscious awareness.
  • It is controlled not only by the focus of our attention but also the expectancies we have prior to exposure
  • Iconic sensory memories (icons) are visual representations that last only about a seconds in sensory memory
  • Echoic sensory memories (echoes) are auditory representations that may last for a few seconds (need a longer period to process language)
  • STM (working memory) is what is in your mind RIGHT NOW.
  • Information in STM only remains there for about 20-30 seconds
  • STM is very sensitive to interruption and interference
  • Brown, Peterson and Peterson (1959) measured the storage capacity of STM
  • Subjects presented with a stimulus and asked to immediately count backwards (rehearsal prevented)
  • By 20 seconds of backwards counting, the previously presented stimulus was forgotten
  • Unless information in STM is important/meaningful or is actively rehearsed, it quickly vanishes from STM and is displaced by whatever comes into our minds next
  • Displacement occurs, then, when new information enters STM and pushes old out
  • STM storage capacity is limited the average adult can hold about 5-9 bits or chunks of information in STM
  • George Miller (1956) Magical Number Seven, plus or minus two
  • Remember the following number
  • 18122001198417891945
  • The number is 18122001198417891945
  • The process of organizing or grouping separate bits of information into larger units or chunks, can increase STM storage capacity.
  • e.g. 1812 2001 1984 1789 1945
  • Memory span is a measurement of STM capacity
  • It measures the largest number of items that can be recalled perfectly from STM after only one presentation
  • No study/rehearsal time is allowed
  • Sometimes used as a component of IQ tests
  • In STM, acoustic coding seems to dominate especially for verbal information
  • Information in STM may be new or retrieved from LTM to be thought about and used.
  • LTM is our permanent storehouse for information
  • It includes all knowledge we have accumulated, all the skills we have learned, and all our memories of past experiences
  • The more meaningful the information, the more easily it can be stored in LTM
  • Unlike STM, LTM seems to have unlimited storage capacity
  • Information in LTM seems to be organized
  • New facts are learned by fitting them into a network of pre-existing knowledge
  • Propositional network theory
  • we store the smallest bits of meaningful information (propositions represented by circles or nodes) and create links (represented by arrows) to other nodes.
  • e.g. the proposition dog may be linked to the other nodes bark, fur, and four legs.
  • Two Broad Types of Memory Circuits
  • Declarative Memory includes facts such as names, dates, and events (sometimes referred to as explicit memory)
  • Can be rapidly learned and forgotten
  • Usually consciously accessed
  • Subdivisions
  • Semantic memory refers specifically to factual information
  • Episodic memory includes our personal or autobiographical experiences
  • Procedural Memory includes skills such as remembering how to ride a bike, play a musical instrument or eat with a fork (sometimes referred to as implicit memory)
  • Typically learned by repetition and practice
  • Difficult to unlearn
  • Often performed without conscious thought
  • Eidetic imagery refers to visual LTMs
  • Eidetic memory is characterized by relatively long-lasting and detailed images of scenes that can be scanned as if the individual were physically present
  • Rare in adults more frequent in children
  • Steven Wiltshire eidetic memory
  • Flashbulb memories are remarkably vivid and seemingly permanent memories
  • typically of highly emotional and personal events in one's life
  • What makes the flashbulb memory special is the emotional arousal at the moment that the event was registered to the memory.
  • Memories are not located in one part of the brain
  • Lashley (1950) tested this
  • created lesions in the brains of rats who had learned a maze.
  • Despite having damaged areas, memory was only weakened, not obliterated
  • Synaptic Changes and neurotransmitters
  • We already know that experience physically changes the brain
  • When learning takes place, more serotonin is released at certain synapses
  • This makes the neurons in this network more likely to fire, as sending neurons are more likely to fire and release neurotransmitters and receiving neurons seem to increase their receptor sites.
  • This process is called Long-Term Potentiation (LTP)
  • ACh also plays a role in memory Alzheimers patients lack of
  • Impact of LTP
  • Drugs that block LTP interfere with learning
  • Drugs that enhance it increase memory ability
  • LTP inhibiting drugs can actually erase recent learning
  • Memory enhancement?
  • CREB protein boosters may help trigger LTP
  • Neurotransmitter glutamate may also enhance LTP
  • ECT (electroconvulsive therapy) and head trauma may disrupt memory and learning as LTP in process is not completed
  • Stress Hormones and Memory
  • When we are excited or stressed, we produce more hormones that make more glucose energy for the brain
  • This leads to increased activity in the amygdala in the limbic system, which is also involved in the formation of memories
  • Stronger emotions stronger memories weaker emotions weaker memories
  • Helps to explain flashbulb memory
  • Hippocampus and explicit memories
  • Responsible for transferring STM to explicit LTM
  • Prolonged exposure to stress hormones can actually shrink the hippocampus and inhibit memory
  • Damage can disrupt movement of information to cortex (LTM)
  • Cerebellum processes implicit memories and classical conditioning (unconscious processes)
  • Retrieval involves accessing information from LTM so that it can be used or examined in STM
  • Retrieval cues help us gain access to a memory
  • Methods of measuring retrieval
  • Recall is when material must be remembered with few or no retrieval cues (free response test)
  • Recognition involves tasks loaded with retrieval cues material must be remembered through identification (e.g. multiple choice test)
  • Relearning indicates the time saved when learning material for the second time (obviously, some learning was remembered)
  • Recognition is far easier than recall we remember more than we can recall
  • The more retrieval cues you have (like strings attached to whatever it is you wish to remember), the more likely you are to recall.
  • The activation of associations in memory often unconscious.
  • Can shape our interpretation of events
  • Group 1 Unscramble the letters to make words
  • Group 2 Unscramble the letters to make words
  • Context can also serve as a retrieval cue
  • Sometimes referred to as locus dependent learning
  • Putting ourselves in the same environment we were in when we learned something may help us to later recall the learned information
  • Helps to explain déjà vu (literally meaning already seen) where similar contexts may trigger memories even when we are in new settings
  • Mood can also have an impact on memory
  • State-dependent memory says that we recall information learned in one state when we do so in that same state
  • e.g. learn information high we may recall it better when high again! (of course sober-sober is BEST!!)
  • Mood-congruent memory says that we tend to recall experiences consistent with our current mood good or bad
  • e.g. we recall how fabulous our childhood was when we are feeling happy, and how heinous it was when depressed
  • People tend to recall the first items (primacy effect) and last items (recency effect) in a list
  • Demonstrates how short- and long-term memory work together
  • Primacy effect reflects long-term memory
  • Recency effect reflects short-term memory
  • While we may curse ourselves for forgetting things, it is good that we can get rid of useless information that would otherwise clutter out thoughts
  • In general we may experience encoding failure, storage decay, and retrieval failure
  • Encoding failure is when information is never really learned it never makes the cut from STM to LTM
  • Storage decay happens when we do not use information in memory and it fades
  • Retrieval failure occurs when there are not enough retrieval cues available to prompt remembering
  • Consolidation failure occurs when disruptions prevent permanent memory from being formed
  • Because of selective attention, we only attend to very little of what we are exposed to
  • Unless there is effort, memories do not form
  • e.g. What does a penny look like?
  • Even after encoding has occurred, sometimes we later forget things
  • Ebbinghaus researched this as well and the results of his experiments yielded the famous forgetting curve
  • The information is there, but we cannot access it!
  • Tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon occurs when we are confident that we know the information but cannot retrieve it due to a lack of retrieval cues. Though we cannot recall it, we can often recognize it.
  • Interference occurs when some information may get in the way of your ability to retrieve other information
  • Proactive interference inability to recall new information due to prior learning
  • Retroactive interference inability to recall older information as a result of new learning
  • Sometimes, prior learning can facilitate the learning of new e.g. knowing the rules of baseball may help in learning softball
  • Distractor studies?
  • Memories new to long-term memory take time to be firmly implanted
  • Disruptions in this process can prevent permanent memory from being formed
  • Retrograde Amnesia loss of memory for events occurring for periods prior to brain injury
  • Anterograde Amnesia loss of memory for events that happen after brain injury
  • Infantile Amnesia failure to consolidate information in memory before age 3, perhaps due to underdeveloped brain and limitations in comprehension
  • Dissociative Fugue-no injury psychosomatic
  • Sometimes we simply forget what happened but why?
  • Many stages of memory processing much can be lost along the way
  • Repression a Freudian defense mechanism that pushes anxiety and guilt-arousing thoughts, feelings and memories out of conscious awareness
  • Many memory researchers believe that repression rarely, if ever, occurs especially if the memory is emotional
  • Gilbert (2006) Information given after an event alters the memory of the event
  • Implications? What you are asked and the way you are asked can lead you to remember the event differently!
  • Loftus research
  • How fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other? OR
  • How fast were the cars going when they hit each other?
  • The question determined the response, though all subjects saw the same video
  • Research on eyewitness testimony?
  • Misinformation Effect
  • When given incorrect information about an event, we tend to remember it incorrectly
  • Even imagining events that did not occur may create false memories
  • Suggesting something happened can make us believe it did when asked to recall later!
  • Source Amnesia attributing memory of an event (real or imagined) to an incorrect source e.g. believing that you experienced something that you only heard about or saw on TV.
  • Reconstructive memory
  • Due to source amnesia and misinformation, we can have false memories we believe are true (fill in gaps when memory fails)
  • This is dangerous when we consider eyewitness testimony
  • Childrens underdeveloped frontal lobes make them more susceptible to false memories - accusation of child abuse?
  • Repressed/recovered memories of abuse?
  • Abuse happens we do not want to dismiss legitimate accusations
  • Forgetting happens especially concerning child abuse when kids may not comprehend what is happening
  • It is normal to recover memories, but when they are retrieved by therapist-aided techniques such as hypnosis of sedation, they are suspect
  • Infantile amnesia (pre-age 3) makes memories before this age unreliable
  • Mnemonics are strategies you may use to improve memory
  • Mnemonists are people with extraordinary memory
  • Rehearsal elaborative rehearsal is better than rote rehearsal!
  • Organization of material in meaningful ways
  • SQ3R survey, question, read, recite, review
  • Overlearning
  • Metamemory be aware of how memory works
  • Spaced practice shorter sessions over an extended period of time better than massed practice
  • Peg Word System image word you associate, then assign new material to each peg visually
  • Minimize interference
  • Maximize retrieval cues state, location
  • Get enough sleep!
  • Cognition all mental activities associated with thinking, including memory, knowing, communicating
  • Cognitive psychologists study all of the following
  • Creating concepts
  • Solving problems
  • Making decisions
  • Forming judgments
  • Mental category or label that represents a class or group of objects, people or events that share common characteristics or qualities.
  • Concepts help us organize our thinking
  • We organize concepts into category hierarchies (cars, cats, flowers, etc.)
  • Artificial concepts refer to those where each member of the concept has all of its defining properties while no non-member does, e.g. squares must have 4 corners and 4 right angles.
  • Natural Concepts have no set defining features but have characteristic features instead, e.g. birds where the object could be a chicken, sparrow or ostrich
  • Members of the concept have some characteristics of it
  • We compare possible members to prototypes, objects/events that typically represent the natural concept
  • Fuzzy concepts?
  • Concepts HELP but dont provide all the answers
  • Steps involved in problem solving
  • Understanding the Problem
  • Planning a solution strategy
  • Carrying out the solution
  • Evaluating progress toward goal/results
  • Problem representation
  • the first step in problem solving can help or hinder
  • how we frame or interpret the problem
  • We can approach problems visually, verbally, mathematically and concretely with objects we may create a matrix to keep track of all possible combinations (LSATs, anyone?)
  • e.g. If we only see the problem of high national debt as a lack of tax revenue, we are limiting ourselves in coming up with other viable solutions to the problem that may be more effective and more appealing to the people.
  • Algorithms - Step-by-step methods that guarantee a solution can be tedious and time consuming
  • Heuristics - Rules of thumb that may help simplify a problem but do not guarantee a solution
  • Insight Aha! moment
  • Hill Climbing - Move progressively closer to goal without moving backward
  • Subgoals or Means-End Analysis - break large problem into smaller, more manageable ones, each of which is easier to solve than the whole problem
  • Working Backwards - Start with a solution/goal and figure out how to get there
  • Trial and Error One solution after another is tested time consuming
  • Incubation Put problem aside and engage in an unrelated task before coming back
  • Expertise/Artificial Intelligence Usually computer programs used to solve specific problems however, sometimes this involves rigid sets that could hinder finding solutions
  • Confirmation Bias
  • Tendency to only consider information that supports preconceived ideas rather than paying attention to contradictory evidence
  • e.g. Gingers are evil only think of Children of the Corn gingers and ignore nice ones
  • e.g. Only strumpets get herpes! - ignore the fact that it only takes one partner to get an STD
  • Mental sets
  • Tendency to perceive a problem that use past experiences to frame the problem a certain way can help or hinder.
  • e.g. 9 dot problem? (must think outside the box!)
  • Functional Fixedness
  • a type of mental set that typically hinders, since you can only see things objects in terms of their customary usage
  • e.g. Record problem
  • Creative problem solving generating solutions that are both unusual and useful
  • Divergent thinking produces many different correct answer to the same question (often creative)
  • Convergent Thinking one correct answer is expected (typically not creative but linear thinking)
  • Brainstorming - a way to get over sets where you use divergent thinking to come up with multiple ideas/possibilities to solve a problem.
  • Remote Association Test (RAT) is one measurement of creativity
  • Requires divergent thinking
  • Modest correlation between creativity and intelligence
  • Highly creative people tend to have above average intelligence, but having a high IQ doesnt guarantee creativity
  • We must make decisions all the time but how?
  • Compensatory Model making a decision by allowing attractive attributes to compensate for unattractive ones (e.g. The car looks all banged up but gets great gas mileage)
  • Non-Compensatory Model does not allow some attributes to offset others (e.g. Dude, that girl is busted I dont care how nice she is I will not date her)
  • Heuristic processes are used when decisions involve a high degree of ambiguity
  • Representativeness heuristic
  • New information is compared to our model of the typical member of a category (prototype)
  • Could lead us to ignore other relevant information
  • e.g. Linda the Bank Teller
  • Availability heuristic
  • Decision is based on information that is most easily retrieved from memory, even if incomplete
  • e.g. More words that start with r or have r as third letter?
  • Overconfidence
  • Our tendency to overestimate the accuracy of our knowledge and judgments
  • e.g. we may believe we can finish a paper/study for a test much more quickly than we actually can.
  • People who are overconfident may often be wrong and the mistakes may be costly but they tend to be happier and feel more comfortable making decisions
  • Belief Perseverance
  • Tendency to cling to our initial beliefs even after these decisions have been discredited
  • The more we justify our initial belief, the more difficult it is to let it go when proven wrong
  • To reduce BP, imagine the opposite perspective
  • Automatic gut reaction not involving explicit reasoning
  • Can lead us to sound decisions, but also careless ones.
  • The manner in which information is presented
  • Research has demonstrated that framing can have a profound impact on decision-making
  • e.g. 95 success rate vs. 5 failure rate a success?
  • 85 lean vs. 15 fat ground beef?
  • High Pressure
  • When decisions are required quickly, experience plays a key role
  • With increased pressure in an emergency situation, decision making often deteriorates and can end in panic
  • Hindsight bias
  • Tendency to view the impact of our decisions as inevitable and predictable after we know the outcome
  • e.g. We would have been miserable together (after deciding to get a divorce)
  • Counterfactual thinking
  • Thinking about alternative realities and things that never happened
  • Often takes the form of If only I had
  • Language is defined as a system of signs and symbols based on specific rules (grammar) used to communicate
  • Very complex human ability
  • A unique ability?
  • Semantics meaning in language
  • Syntax rules that determine how words are combined in a language
  • Phonetics how sounds are put together to form words
  • Grammar culmination of rules for generating language (includes phonetics and syntax)
  • Pragmatics social aspects of language (politeness, conversational rules)
  • Psycholinguistics study of the psychological mechanisms related to language acquisition
  • Top Down Processing vs. Bottom Up Processing?
  • TD Meaning/Thought ? production of sounds
  • BU Sounds ? derive meaning
  • Nature vs. Nurture?
  • Critical periods
  • Noam Chomsky (Nature)
  • Language Acquisition Device (LAD)
  • Surface Structure vs. Deep Structure
  • Transformational Grammar Theory
  • Skinners learning theory (Nurture)
  • Linguistic Determinism (Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis) - Language determines thought
  • Linguistic Relativity Thoughts/experiences determine language
  • Cooing/Cruing
  • Holophrase and 1-word speech
  • 2-word speech
  • Telegraphic Speech doggie bite face!
  • Verbs and modifiers added
  • Syntax acquired
  • Overgeneralization/overextension
  • Facial expressions and Paul Ekmans work
  • Emblems (gestures) and body language
  • Do animals think?
  • Animals are capable of more than we thought!
  • Forming concepts? Even pigeons!
  • Insight (Kohler)
  • Numerical ability (arithmetic)
  • Transmission of cultural patterns (primates)
  • Self Awareness
  • AJ the fabulous bird
  • Can animals talk?
  • They DO communicate, but is it language?
  • Primate Language
  • Use of signs and symbols
  • Novel combinations of signs would indicate a higher level of cognitive processing
  • Vocalizations with different meanings
  • Gestured communication/facial expressions
  • Koko the Gorilla, Washoe the Chimp, Kanzi the bonobo

PowerShow.com is a leading presentation sharing website. It has millions of presentations already uploaded and available with 1,000s more being uploaded by its users every day. Whatever your area of interest, here you’ll be able to find and view presentations you’ll love and possibly download. And, best of all, it is completely free and easy to use.

You might even have a presentation you’d like to share with others. If so, just upload it to PowerShow.com. We’ll convert it to an HTML5 slideshow that includes all the media types you’ve already added: audio, video, music, pictures, animations and transition effects. Then you can share it with your target audience as well as PowerShow.com’s millions of monthly visitors. And, again, it’s all free.

About the Developers

PowerShow.com is brought to you by  CrystalGraphics , the award-winning developer and market-leading publisher of rich-media enhancement products for presentations. Our product offerings include millions of PowerPoint templates, diagrams, animated 3D characters and more.

World's Best PowerPoint Templates PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Got any suggestions?

We want to hear from you! Send us a message and help improve Slidesgo

Top searches

Trending searches

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

49 templates

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

18 templates

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

32 templates

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

42 templates

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

40 templates

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

16 templates

Problem Solving and Mathematical Reasoning

Problem solving and mathematical reasoning presentation, free google slides theme, powerpoint template, and canva presentation template.

Let's make math learning more fun, especially at early levels of education. This new template has some cute illustrations and lots of elements related to math, including backgrounds that look like blackboards. This is a great choice for teachers who want to turn their classes into a more entertaining experience for their students. Customize the slides to add your own activities or explanations!

Features of this template

  • 100% editable and easy to modify
  • 36 different slides to impress your audience
  • Contains easy-to-edit graphics such as graphs, maps, tables, timelines and mockups
  • Includes 500+ icons and Flaticon’s extension for customizing your slides
  • Designed to be used in Google Slides, Canva, and Microsoft PowerPoint
  • 16:9 widescreen format suitable for all types of screens
  • Includes information about fonts, colors, and credits of the free resources used

How can I use the template?

Am I free to use the templates?

How to attribute?

Combines with:

This template can be combined with this other one to create the perfect presentation:

Problem Solving and Mathematical Reasoning Infographics

Attribution required If you are a free user, you must attribute Slidesgo by keeping the slide where the credits appear. How to attribute?

Related posts on our blog.

How to Add, Duplicate, Move, Delete or Hide Slides in Google Slides | Quick Tips & Tutorial for your presentations

How to Add, Duplicate, Move, Delete or Hide Slides in Google Slides

How to Change Layouts in PowerPoint | Quick Tips & Tutorial for your presentations

How to Change Layouts in PowerPoint

How to Change the Slide Size in Google Slides | Quick Tips & Tutorial for your presentations

How to Change the Slide Size in Google Slides

Related presentations.

Problem Solving and Mathematical Reasoning Infographics presentation template

Premium template

Unlock this template and gain unlimited access

Problem-Solving and Reasoning - Math - 5th Grade presentation template

SlidePlayer

  • My presentations

Auth with social network:

Download presentation

We think you have liked this presentation. If you wish to download it, please recommend it to your friends in any social system. Share buttons are a little bit lower. Thank you!

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Cognition: Thinking and Language

Published by Marilynn Harrison Modified over 5 years ago

Similar presentations

Presentation on theme: "Cognition: Thinking and Language"— Presentation transcript:

Cognition: Thinking and Language

HOLT, RINEHART AND WINSTON P SYCHOLOGY PRINCIPLES IN PRACTICE 1 Chapter 8 THINKING AND LANGUAGE Section 1: What Is Thinking?What Is Thinking? Section 2:

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Thinking and Problem Solving. Cognition Cognition – the mental activities associated with thinking, knowing, remembering, and communicating How do we.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Thinking and language. -thinking about our own thinking Examples: 1. Learning about your own best style of learning. 2. Becoming aware of your own biases.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Cognitive Psychology Chapter 7. Cognitive Psychology: Overview  Cognitive psychology is the study of perception, learning, memory, and thought  The.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Cognition Thoughts, Beliefs, and Attitudes. Moving from thoughts to behavior Concepts Propositions Behavior Mental Models.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Chapter 8 Thinking, Intelligence, and Language

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Warm Up Answers 3. YYURYYUBICURYY4ME Coffin

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

DO NOW:  What is cognition (it’s okay to guess)?  Prepare your spring break extra credit to turn in (if you have it).

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Thinking, Intelligence, and Language

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

© 2009 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. C H A P T E R 9 Complex Cognitive Processes.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Chapter 10 Thinking and Language.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Wyatt Andresen, M’Kyla Walker, Sarah Kerman, Jake Garn, Chris Pirrung.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

C OGNITION Thinking, Problem Solving, Creativity and Language.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

What is “Thinking”? Forming ideas Drawing conclusions Expressing thoughts Comprehending the thoughts of others Where does it occur? Distributed throughout.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Unit 7B Thinking, Problem Solving, Creativity, and Language Monday, November 18, 2013.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY EIGHTH EDITION IN MODULES David Myers PowerPoint Slides Aneeq Ahmad Henderson State University Worth Publishers, © 2011.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Myers EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY (6th Edition in Modules) Module 23 Thinking James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers.

memory thinking and problem solving ppt

Thinking and Problem Solving Cognition Cognition – the mental activities associated with thinking, knowing, remembering, and communicating.

About project

© 2024 SlidePlayer.com Inc. All rights reserved.

thinking and problem solving

Thinking and Problem Solving

Jul 22, 2014

70 likes | 261 Views

Thinking and Problem Solving. What is thinking?. Thinking is the mental activity that is involved in the manipulation and understanding of information. Units of Thought: Concept : group of objects, events, or ideas that share similar characteristics Concept of “mom,” “dad,” “soccer game”

Share Presentation

  • functional fixedness
  • soccer game
  • often novel
  • usual functions

clancy

Presentation Transcript

What is thinking? • Thinking is the mental activity that is involved in the manipulation and understanding of information. • Units of Thought: • Concept: group of objects, events, or ideas that share similar characteristics • Concept of “mom,” “dad,” “soccer game” • Prototype: an example of a concept that best exemplifies that concept (most typical example of our concepts) • Schemas: organized approach to answering questions or solving problems

How do we Problem Solve? • Algorithm: when used properly right answer every time, sequence of events • Formulas • Light doesn’t work (what steps are logical to take?)

How do we Problem Solve? • Heuristic: “Rules of thumb” – often, not always work like a mental shortcut • Faster than algorithms, but not as reliable • Representative Heuristic: Making decisions about a sample according to the population that the sample appears to represent • (judging based on how similar the aspects are to prototypes we have – ex: depressed teens and suicide rates) • Availability Heuristic: making decisions based on the information available • (judging based on examples of similar situations – ex: familiarity with neighborhoods)

How do we Problem Solve? • Insight: A sudden and often novel realization of the solution to a problem: • Example : Kohler’s apes

What are obstacles to Problem Solving? • Framing: the way in which the wording influences a decision • Leading Questions… • Fixation: the inability to see a problem from a fresh perspective – fixed on only one part of the problem • Functional Fixedness: the tendency to think of things only in terms of their usual functions (not creative, get stuck on literal interpretation)

  • More by User

Problem Solving and Creative Thinking

Problem Solving and Creative Thinking

Problem Solving and Creative Thinking. Problem Solving. What is a Problem? A problem arises when a living creature has a goal but does not know how this goal is to be reached.

2.22k views • 61 slides

Questioning, Thinking, and Problem Solving

Questioning, Thinking, and Problem Solving

Questioning, Thinking, and Problem Solving. Tiffany Hogan. Journaling Choice Board (Please select 1-2). Thinking, questioning, problem solving. Be a 5!. Teachers’ lounge. “Wow! I had no idea aspirin came in such large bottles!”. Introduction. About Me Parking Lot Chocolate

1.22k views • 84 slides

Problem Solving and Critical Thinking

Problem Solving and Critical Thinking

Problem Solving and Critical Thinking. Mohamed Elhammoumi Department of Psychology Imam University. What is Abstract Thinking?.

769 views • 16 slides

Thinking and Problem Solving

Thinking and Problem Solving. The Basics of Thinking. Thinking: changing reorganizing, and using info stored in your memory to create new information Convergent Thinking : systematic attempt to reach a specific goal/answer. Used to solve math problems and accomplish personal goals.

276 views • 12 slides

Computational Thinking, Problem-Solving and Programming

Computational Thinking, Problem-Solving and Programming

Topic 4. Computational Thinking, Problem-Solving and Programming. Assessment Objectives. Assessment objective 1 : classify, define, draw, label, list, state Assessment objective 2 : annotate, apply, calculate, describe, design, distinguish, estimate, identify, outline , present, trace

888 views • 27 slides

Thinking and Problem Solving

Thinking and Problem Solving. Week 2: Making Connections . The TAP TM System Training Portal. Objective. Teachers will connect thinking and problem solving and develop a lesson incorporating both the process and the product. . Agenda. Evaluation Long Range Plan & Thinking Evaluation

864 views • 18 slides

Thinking and Problem Solving

Thinking and Problem Solving. “Our job is not to make up anyone’s mind, but to open minds and to make the agony of decision-making so intense, you can only escape by thinking.” - Author Unknown. The TAP TM System Training Portal. Reflective Thinking… How did you implement?.

360 views • 16 slides

Critical Thinking/Problem Solving

Critical Thinking/Problem Solving

Critical Thinking/Problem Solving. Tammy LeJune. Critical Thinking/Problem Solving. How to Incorporate Critical Thinking Skills in the Classroom. Teaching Grammar Traditionally:

427 views • 7 slides

Thinking & Problem Solving

Thinking & Problem Solving

Thinking & Problem Solving. http://www.starlight-tower.com/images/starlight_tower/The_Thinker_Rodin-2.jpg. Studying Problem Solving. Types of problems Well-defined problems Ill-defined problems. Studying Problem Solving. Methods Logic puzzles Introspection. Classic Problems and Solutions.

417 views • 22 slides

Critical Thinking, Problem Solving, and Critical Reading

Critical Thinking, Problem Solving, and Critical Reading

Critical Thinking, Problem Solving, and Critical Reading. JCTC-DT READING/ACADEMIC SUCCSS GOAL 2001-2002 Use Critical Thinking and Problem Solving skills to learn and use with the content in the Study Skills/Academic Success Courses and

426 views • 7 slides

Thinking: Problem Solving and Decision Making

Thinking: Problem Solving and Decision Making

Thinking: Problem Solving and Decision Making. Units of Thoughts. Concept A mental grouping based on shared similarity Categorizing items in one’s environment Prototype A typical best example incorporating the major features of a concept

556 views • 24 slides

Chapter 15: Critical Thinking and Problem Solving

Chapter 15: Critical Thinking and Problem Solving

Chapter 15: Critical Thinking and Problem Solving. Critical Thinking. Definition & Characteristics (Dr. Richard Paul) “ A systematic way to form and shape one ’ s thinking ” Functions purposefully & exactingly Disciplined Comprehensive Based on intellectual standards Well-reasoned.

550 views • 20 slides

Thinking and Problem Solving

Thinking and Problem Solving. Fact #1. The brain is awesome – and we know nothing about it. No, but really… there are reasons she will be forever alone. Girl’s got some moves?. How is all that possible – and you can speak and pick up a pencil “…without thinking about it?”.

391 views • 24 slides

Problem Solving and Algorithmic Thinking

Problem Solving and Algorithmic Thinking

Standard BCS-CMW 17,18,19. Problem Solving and Algorithmic Thinking. Problem Solving Steps (algorithm). Recognize that something is wrong ID the actual problem (root cause) Generate alternative solutions Choose from the alternative solutions Implement the chosen solution

342 views • 20 slides

Critical Thinking and Problem-solving

Critical Thinking and Problem-solving

Critical Thinking and Problem-solving. NURUL SYAFIQAH NAJWA BT NU MAHAMAD A151683 MOBILE APPLICATION DEVELOPMENT (MAD) CLUB. Introduction. Problem : Majority of the UKM students, especially the first-year students have problems related with the bus.

153 views • 13 slides

Critical Thinking and Problem Solving

Critical Thinking and Problem Solving

Critical Thinking and Problem Solving. Clinic Sessions – Understanding the Rationale for the Elements of 21 st Century Curriculum. Cheryl Lemke Metiri Group. Critical Thinking & Problem Solving. TODAY What is it? Why is it important?. TOMORROW

399 views • 37 slides

Thinking & Problem Solving

Thinking & Problem Solving. Thinking. Thought. Thinking—changing and reorganizing the information stored in memory to create new information Mental processes directed at a goal or purpose. 5 kinds of units of thought: Image Symbol Concepts Prototype rule. Image.

541 views • 41 slides

Problem Solving and Critical Thinking

Problem Solving and Critical Thinking. Presentation outline. Why teaching Critical Thinking and Problem Solving is important Nature of Critical Thinking and Problem Solving Characteristics of an effective Critical Thinker Challenges to teaching Critical Thinking.

393 views • 20 slides

Problem Solving  and Strategic Thinking

Problem Solving and Strategic Thinking

Problem Solving and Strategic Thinking. Welcome. Welcome! Facilitator: Tracy Laycock. Identifying Problems. What is a Problem?. Difficulties. Emotions Mental shortcuts Need for an explanation Biased evidence. Pitfalls to Problem Solving. Understand the Steps. Define the Problem.

314 views • 26 slides

DECISION MAKING, PROBLEM SOLVING, AND CRITICAL THINKING

DECISION MAKING, PROBLEM SOLVING, AND CRITICAL THINKING

DECISION MAKING, PROBLEM SOLVING, AND CRITICAL THINKING. DECISION MAKING, PROBLEM SOLVING, AND CRITICAL THINKING. why director of nursing at KAH took a decision to start an in- service educational for nurses?

1.25k views • 44 slides

IMAGES

  1. PPT

    memory thinking and problem solving ppt

  2. Problem Solving Powerpoint Template Free

    memory thinking and problem solving ppt

  3. PSYCHOLOGY-Thinking and Problem Solving

    memory thinking and problem solving ppt

  4. PPT

    memory thinking and problem solving ppt

  5. PPT

    memory thinking and problem solving ppt

  6. 4 Steps Problem Solving Template

    memory thinking and problem solving ppt

VIDEO

  1. AI: The Shocking Future That Will Change Everything You Know!

  2. Memory Game

  3. PPT ||Topic- Computer Memory||

  4. memory problem solved 💯👈🏼

  5. Chapter 3 Problem Solving (part 1)

  6. Holographic Memory Seminar Report ppt download

COMMENTS

  1. Memory and Thinking

    3 likes • 3,000 views. Kate Marielle Balueta. Visuals for General Psychology Class Reporting. Education Technology. 1 of 24. Download Now. Download to read offline. Memory and Thinking - Download as a PDF or view online for free.

  2. Unit 5

    Unit 5: Cognition 5.1 Introduction to Memory 5.2 Encoding 5.3 Storing 5.4 Retrieving 5.5 Forgetting and Memory Distortion 5.6 Biological Bases for Memory 5.7 Introduction to Thinking and Problem Solving 5.8 Biases and Errors in Thinking 5.9 Introduction to Intelligence 5.10 Psychometric Principle...

  3. PPT Memory

    Operations: Rules that determine how the information in the memory system is utilized (reasoning, problem solving, and logical analysis). Take math as an example: Is I have 100 numbers stored in my memory and am confronted with a mathematical problem, these operation determine how I solve the problem.

  4. Unit 3: Learning, Memory, Thinking, and Language

    Cognition Unit 6: We are here Algorithms Heuristics Biological Factors Representativeness Heuristic Compensatory Models Problem Solving Techniques Decision Making Techniques Availability Heuristic Unit 6: Cognition Obstacles to Problem Solving We are here Obstacles to Decision Making Biological Factors Acquisition and use of Language Memory Information Processing Model Encoding Storage ...

  5. 7 Module 7: Thinking, Reasoning, and Problem-Solving

    Module 7: Thinking, Reasoning, and Problem-Solving. This module is about how a solid working knowledge of psychological principles can help you to think more effectively, so you can succeed in school and life. You might be inclined to believe that—because you have been thinking for as long as you can remember, because you are able to figure ...

  6. Cognitive Processes: Memory and Problem Solving

    Presentation Transcript. Cognitive Processes:Memory and Problem Solving Chapters 8 and 9. Memory Defined and The Information Processing Model • Defining Memory • The persistence of learning over time through storage and retrieval of information • Much of what we have learned about memory comes from cases of memory loss or extraordinary ...

  7. Introduction to Thinking and Problem-Solving

    This is only one facet of the complex processes involved in cognition. Simply put, cognition is thinking, and it encompasses the processes associated with perception, knowledge, problem solving, judgment, language, and memory. Scientists who study cognition are searching for ways to understand how we integrate, organize, and utilize our ...

  8. 11.4: Information Processing- Learning, Memory, and Problem Solving

    Figure 11.4.1 11.4. 1: Working memory expands during middle and late childhood. ( Image by Anchor is licensed under CC BY-NC-SA) Attention: As noted above, the ability to inhibit irrelevant information improves during this age group, with there being a sharp improvement in selective attention from age six into adolescence (Vakil, Blachstein ...

  9. Cognitive Psychology: The Science of How We Think

    MaskotOwner/Getty Images. Cognitive psychology involves the study of internal mental processes—all of the workings inside your brain, including perception, thinking, memory, attention, language, problem-solving, and learning. Cognitive psychology--the study of how people think and process information--helps researchers understand the human brain.

  10. PPT

    Cognitive Processes: Memory and Problem Solving - PowerPoint PPT Presentation. Actions. Remove this presentation Flag as Inappropriate I Don't Like This I like this Remember as a Favorite. ... Cognition Memory Language Thinking Problem Solving and Creativity Memory: 3 storage units: Sensory perception to memory; iconic ...

  11. PPT

    Thinking and Problem Solving. The Basics of Thinking • Thinking: changing reorganizing, and using info stored in your memory to create new information • Convergent Thinking: systematic attempt to reach a specific goal/answer. • Used to solve math problems and accomplish personal goals. • Ex. What is 2 X 5? • Divergent Thinking: free flow of thoughts with no particular plan.

  12. Cognition in Psychology: Definition, Types, Effects, and Tips

    Cognition is a term referring to the mental processes involved in gaining knowledge and comprehension. Some of the many different cognitive processes include thinking, knowing, remembering, judging, and problem-solving. These are higher-level functions of the brain and encompass language, imagination, perception, and planning.

  13. Critical Thinking Lesson Google Slides & PowerPoint template

    Free Google Slides theme and PowerPoint template. Critical thinking appears to be a difficult matter to understand. Well, we can help you with the design of a presentation about that topic! You'll notice the colorful nature of the elements used in the slides, which may encourage viewers (or students!) to think about this concept. Try this ...

  14. PPT

    Presentation Transcript. Thinking & Problem Solving. Thinking. Thought • Thinking—changing and reorganizing the information stored in memory to create new information • Mental processes directed at a goal or purpose. • 5 kinds of units of thought: • Image • Symbol • Concepts • Prototype • rule. Image • Image -a visual ...

  15. PPT

    Cognitive Processes: Memory and Problem Solving - PowerPoint PPT Presentation. 1 / 67 . Actions. Remove this presentation Flag as Inappropriate I Don't Like This I like this Remember as a Favorite. ... Cognition Memory Language Thinking Problem Solving and Creativity Memory: 3 storage units: Sensory perception to memory; iconic ...

  16. Problem Solving & Mathematical Reasoning

    Free Google Slides theme, PowerPoint template, and Canva presentation template. Let's make math learning more fun, especially at early levels of education. This new template has some cute illustrations and lots of elements related to math, including backgrounds that look like blackboards. This is a great choice for teachers who want to turn ...

  17. Problem Solving Powerpoint Templates and Google Slides Themes

    Download your presentation as a PowerPoint template or use it online as a Google Slides theme. 100% free, no registration or download limits. Get these problem-solving templates to create effective presentations that offer solutions to any challenge. No Download Limits Free for Any Use No Signups.

  18. Cognition: Thinking and Language

    8 Problem Solving Obstacles Fixation: Using a prior strategy and failing to look at a problem from a fresh new perspective. When we are unable to see a problem from a different perspective it hinders problem solving Mental Set: Tendency to approach a problem in a certain way, because that method worked before. Rigidity Functional Fixedness: Failing to solve a problem as a result of fixation on ...

  19. PPT

    Presentation Transcript. What is thinking? • Thinking is the mental activity that is involved in the manipulation and understanding of information. • Units of Thought: • Concept: group of objects, events, or ideas that share similar characteristics • Concept of "mom," "dad," "soccer game" • Prototype: an example of a ...