Library homepage

  • school Campus Bookshelves
  • menu_book Bookshelves
  • perm_media Learning Objects
  • login Login
  • how_to_reg Request Instructor Account
  • hub Instructor Commons
  • Download Page (PDF)
  • Download Full Book (PDF)
  • Periodic Table
  • Physics Constants
  • Scientific Calculator
  • Reference & Cite
  • Tools expand_more
  • Readability

selected template will load here

This action is not available.

Mathematics LibreTexts

2.10: Problem Solving and Decision Making

  • Last updated
  • Save as PDF
  • Page ID 41786

Learning Objectives

  • Learn to understand the problem.
  • Learn to combine creative thinking and critical thinking to solve problems.
  • Practice problem solving in a group.

Much of your college and professional life will be spent solving problems; some will be complex, such as deciding on a career, and require time and effort to come up with a solution. Others will be small, such as deciding what to eat for lunch, and will allow you to make a quick decision based entirely on your own experience. But, in either case, when coming up with the solution and deciding what to do, follow the same basic steps.

  • Define the problem. Use your analytical skills. What is the real issue? Why is it a problem? What are the root causes? What kinds of outcomes or actions do you expect to generate to solve the problem? What are some of the key characteristics that will make a good choice: Timing? Resources? Availability of tools and materials? For more complex problems, it helps to actually write out the problem and the answers to these questions. Can you clarify your understanding of the problem by using metaphors to illustrate the issue?
  • Narrow the problem. Many problems are made up of a series of smaller problems, each requiring its own solution. Can you break the problem into different facets? What aspects of the current issue are “noise” that should not be considered in the problem solution? (Use critical thinking to separate facts from opinion in this step.)
  • Generate possible solutions. List all your options. Use your creative thinking skills in this phase. Did you come up with the second “right” answer, and the third or the fourth? Can any of these answers be combined into a stronger solution? What past or existing solutions can be adapted or combined to solve this problem?

Group Think: Effective Brainstorming

Brainstorming is a process of generating ideas for solutions in a group. This method is very effective because ideas from one person will trigger additional ideas from another. The following guidelines make for an effective brainstorming session:

  • Decide who should moderate the session. That person may participate, but his main role is to keep the discussion flowing.
  • Define the problem to be discussed and the time you will allow to consider it.
  • Write all ideas down on a board or flip chart for all participants to see.
  • Encourage everyone to speak.
  • Do not allow criticism of ideas. All ideas are good during a brainstorm. Suspend disbelief until after the session. Remember a wildly impossible idea may trigger a creative and feasible solution to a problem.
  • Choose the best solution. Use your critical thinking skills to select the most likely choices. List the pros and cons for each of your selections. How do these lists compare with the requirements you identified when you defined the problem? If you still can’t decide between options, you may want to seek further input from your brainstorming team.

Decisions, Decisions

You will be called on to make many decisions in your life. Some will be personal, like what to major in, or whether or not to get married. Other times you will be making decisions on behalf of others at work or for a volunteer organization. Occasionally you will be asked for your opinion or experience for decisions others are making. To be effective in all of these circumstances, it is helpful to understand some principles about decision making.

First, define who is responsible for solving the problem or making the decision. In an organization, this may be someone above or below you on the organization chart but is usually the person who will be responsible for implementing the solution. Deciding on an academic major should be your decision, because you will have to follow the course of study. Deciding on the boundaries of a sales territory would most likely be the sales manager who supervises the territories, because he or she will be responsible for producing the results with the combined territories. Once you define who is responsible for making the decision, everyone else will fall into one of two roles: giving input, or in rare cases, approving the decision.

Understanding the role of input is very important for good decisions. Input is sought or given due to experience or expertise, but it is up to the decision maker to weigh the input and decide whether and how to use it. Input should be fact based, or if offering an opinion, it should be clearly stated as such. Finally, once input is given, the person giving the input must support the other’s decision, whether or not the input is actually used.

Consider a team working on a project for a science course. The team assigns you the responsibility of analyzing and presenting a large set of complex data. Others on the team will set up the experiment to demonstrate the hypothesis, prepare the class presentation, and write the paper summarizing the results. As you face the data, you go to the team to seek input about the level of detail on the data you should consider for your analysis. The person doing the experiment setup thinks you should be very detailed, because then it will be easy to compare experiment results with the data. However, the person preparing the class presentation wants only high-level data to be considered because that will make for a clearer presentation. If there is not a clear understanding of the decision-making process, each of you may think the decision is yours to make because it influences the output of your work; there will be conflict and frustration on the team. If the decision maker is clearly defined upfront, however, and the input is thoughtfully given and considered, a good decision can be made (perhaps a creative compromise?) and the team can get behind the decision and work together to complete the project.

Finally, there is the approval role in decisions. This is very common in business decisions but often occurs in college work as well (the professor needs to approve the theme of the team project, for example). Approval decisions are usually based on availability of resources, legality, history, or policy.

Key Takeaways

  • Effective problem solving involves critical and creative thinking.

The four steps to effective problem solving are the following:

  • Define the problem
  • Narrow the problem
  • Generate solutions
  • Choose the solution
  • Brainstorming is a good method for generating creative solutions.
  • Understanding the difference between the roles of deciding and providing input makes for better decisions.

Checkpoint Exercises

Gather a group of three or four friends and conduct three short brainstorming sessions (ten minutes each) to generate ideas for alternate uses for peanut butter, paper clips, and pen caps. Compare the results of the group with your own ideas. Be sure to follow the brainstorming guidelines. Did you generate more ideas in the group? Did the quality of the ideas improve? Were the group ideas more innovative? Which was more fun? Write your conclusions here.

__________________________________________________________________

Using the steps outlined earlier for problem solving, write a plan for the following problem: You are in your second year of studies in computer animation at Jefferson Community College. You and your wife both work, and you would like to start a family in the next year or two. You want to become a video game designer and can benefit from more advanced work in programming. Should you go on to complete a four-year degree?

Define the problem: What is the core issue? What are the related issues? Are there any requirements to a successful solution? Can you come up with a metaphor to describe the issue?

Narrow the problem: Can you break down the problem into smaller manageable pieces? What would they be?

Generate solutions: What are at least two “right” answers to each of the problem pieces?

Choose the right approach: What do you already know about each solution? What do you still need to know? How can you get the information you need? Make a list of pros and cons for each solution.

Mathematics and algorithms for intelligent decision-making

A meeting in a modern officespace.

There are many benefits of using mathematics and algorithms to aid decision-making in complex settings. Our main focus is on discrete optimisation for decision problems that aim to schedule or allocate resources. In our research projects, we both find new ways to address decision problems by optimisation and develop new methods for solving them.     

Artificial intelligence

Thanks to digitalisation, the available amount of data to support decision-making has reached a high level of maturity, and so has the capability of processing this data. Alongside the data science disciplines, optimisation — used for decision support — plays a key role in the further development of tools to support advanced decision-making. The scale and complexity of the problems relevant to address continue to increase and this calls for research that address the mathematics and algorithms needed in optimisation methods.

Intelligent decision-making

We believe that truly intelligent decision-making is achieved through an integration of model-based and data-driven approaches that are designed or used in interaction with a human decision-maker. To successfully apply mathematics and algorithms as part of real-world decision-making requires careful mathematical modelling and data collection. An inherent property of many decision problems of practical relevance is that they are computationally challenging. Solving such a problem within a reasonable amount of time often requires the development of specialised methods that exploit the mathematical structure of the problem.

More information

On the journey towards sustainability, our contribution is to develop mathematical models and solution methods for practically relevant but computationally challenging problems in scheduling and resource allocation.

Discrete optimisation

There are many types of decision problems that aim to schedule or allocate resources, and these lend themselves to be addressed as discrete optimisation problems. Most practically relevant discrete optimisation problems are NP-hard, which means that their worst-case solution times grow exponentially with the problem size. The practical consequence of this is that, for challenging instances, even the state-of-the-art optimisation solvers will frequently fail to deliver a solution within weeks or even thousands of years of computational time.

Over the last decades, there has been an impressive development of methods for solving discrete optimisation problems. Thanks to this, many important planning and scheduling problems can be solved with a reasonable computational effort—however, many practically relevant problems still pose great challenges.

Organisation

This research direction, formerly called  Operations Research Methods for Scheduling and Resource Allocation Problems  has been established through funding from the Center for Industrial Information Technology (CENIIT) and is carried out at the Division of Applied Mathematics (TIMA) in the Department of Mathematics (MAI). Leader of the group is Elina Rönnberg.

Some of our applied projects and PhD thesis projects are highlighted in the list of research projects below.

Ongoing projects

Airplanes.

Resource allocation and scheduling for future avionic systems

To fully utilize the potential in modern modular avionic systems, very difficult allocation and scheduling problems may have to be solved. In cooperation with Saab Aeronautics, we develop specialized solution methods for future sysems.

Electrical truck.

Route planning of heavy-duty electric vehicles

Electrification of heavy-duty vehicles calls for intelligent methods to optimize the planning of their use and charging. With Scania and Ragn-Sells we develop mathematical models and algorithms for the next generation of transportation systems.

Tegel airport in Berlin

Some of the planning tasks involved in air traffic management can be considered as optimisation problems. These are typically large scale and difficult to solve.

Three computer screens.

ELLIIT PhD student project

Discrete optimisation for automatic decision-making in large-scale complex systems, in collaboration with Lund University.

Previous projects

A SJ-train driving through a beautiful landscape.

Decision support for railway crew planning

For our society to fully reap the benefits of sustainable train travel, careful resource planning is essential. Passengers need to be able to rely on that trains are on time and train operators need to make efficient use of their vehicles and crew.

Pilot controls the plane in the cockpit.

Scheduling of Avionic Systems

In an avionic system, communications and activities need to be scheduled to ensure the functionality of the aeroplane. Efficient methods for such scheduling are of importance for the development of future avionic systems.

Instrument panels inside the cockpit of an airplane.

Operations Research Methods for Scheduling and Dimensioning of Real-Time Systems

Decision problems for dynamic systems with timeliness or performance requirements can be formulated as discrete optimization problems.

Telecommunication tower on blue sky background.

Decision Support for Scheduling: Models, Methods and Validation

In this project we have developed column generation methods for a kind of scheduling in telecommunication and for solving transportation problems with both fixed and linear costs.

Silver metal briefcase against white background.

Mean-Variance Portfolio Optimization

We have developed solution methods for computationally challenging large scale instances of Markowitz's mean-variance portfolio optimization problem. We are also studying the cardinality-constrained version of the problem.

Photo of Elina Rönnberg

Elina Rönnberg

Deputy Head of Department, Senior Associate Professor

  • Department of Mathematics (MAI)
  • Applied Mathematics (TIMA)
  • elina.ronnberg@ liu.se
  • +4613281645

Researchers

Photo of Elina Rönnberg

Aban Ansari-Önnestam

PhD student

Photo of Roghayeh Hajizadeh

Roghayeh Hajizadeh

Photo of Lukas Eveborn

Lukas Eveborn

Collaborations and co-supervision network

Photo of Daniel Jung

Daniel Jung

Associate Professor, Docent

mathematics in problem solving and decision making

Svante Johansson

Photo of Christiane Schmidt

Christiane Schmidt

Senior Associate Professor

Aigerim Saken

PhD Student

Stephen J. Maher

Senior Lecturer

Günther Raidl

Associate Professor

Anders Forsgren

Photo of Susanna F. de Rezende

Susanna F. de Rezende

Assistant Professor

Related research

Working methodology for optimisation: Problem-Model-Calculations-Evaluation-Decision

Optimization Aims at Finding the Best Solutions to Difficult Problems

Activities in society and industry must be based on good decisions to be efficient and economical. Optimization is about identifying, formulating and solving large, complex problems.

  • Operations research methods for scheduling and resource allocation problems
  • Optimization
  • Mathematics
  • Space and Aviation
  • Research Activity
  • AI Artificiell intelligens

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here .

Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

Does mathematics training lead to better logical thinking and reasoning? A cross-sectional assessment from students to professors

Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

Affiliation School of Mathematics and Statistics, The University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia

Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

* E-mail: [email protected]

Affiliation School of Arts and Humanities, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, Australia

ORCID logo

  • Clio Cresswell, 
  • Craig P. Speelman

PLOS

  • Published: July 29, 2020
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153
  • Peer Review
  • Reader Comments

Fig 1

Mathematics is often promoted as endowing those who study it with transferable skills such as an ability to think logically and critically or to have improved investigative skills, resourcefulness and creativity in problem solving. However, there is scant evidence to back up such claims. This project tested participants with increasing levels of mathematics training on 11 well-studied rational and logical reasoning tasks aggregated from various psychological studies. These tasks, that included the Cognitive Reflection Test and the Wason Selection Task, are of particular interest as they have typically and reliably eluded participants in all studies, and results have been uncorrelated with general intelligence, education levels and other demographic information. The results in this study revealed that in general the greater the mathematics training of the participant, the more tasks were completed correctly, and that performance on some tasks was also associated with performance on others not traditionally associated. A ceiling effect also emerged. The work is deconstructed from the viewpoint of adding to the platform from which to approach the greater, and more scientifically elusive, question: are any skills associated with mathematics training innate or do they arise from skills transfer?

Citation: Cresswell C, Speelman CP (2020) Does mathematics training lead to better logical thinking and reasoning? A cross-sectional assessment from students to professors. PLoS ONE 15(7): e0236153. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153

Editor: Jérôme Prado, French National Center for Scientific Research (CNRS) & University of Lyon, FRANCE

Received: January 13, 2020; Accepted: June 30, 2020; Published: July 29, 2020

Copyright: © 2020 Cresswell, Speelman. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding: The authors received no specific funding for this work.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Mathematics is often promoted as endowing those who study it with a number of broad thinking skills such as: an ability to think logically, analytically, critically and abstractly; having capacity to weigh evidence with impartiality. This is a view of mathematics as providing transferable skills which can be found across educational institutions, governments and corporations worldwide. A view material to the place of mathematics in curricula.

Consider the UK government’s commissioned inquiry into mathematics education “Making Mathematics Count” ascertaining the justification that “mathematical training disciplines the mind, develops logical and critical reasoning, and develops analytical and problem-solving skills to a high degree” [ 1 p11]. The Australian Mathematical Sciences Institute very broadly states in its policy document “Vision for a Maths Nation” that “Not only is mathematics the enabling discipline, it has a vital productive role planning and protecting our well-being” (emphasis in original) [ 2 ]. In Canada, British Columbia’s New 2016 curriculum K-9 expressly mentions as part of its “Goals and Rationale”: “The Mathematics program of study is designed to develop deep mathematical understanding and fluency, logical reasoning, analytical thought, and creative thinking.” [ 3 ]. Universities, too, often make such specific claims with respect to their teaching programs. “Mathematics and statistics will help you to think logically and clearly, and apply a range of problem-solving strategies” is claimed by The School of Mathematical Sciences at Monash University, Australia [ 4 ]. The School of Mathematics and Statistics at The University of Sydney, Australia, directly attributes as part of particular course objectives and outcomes skills that include “enhance your problem-solving skills” as part of studies in first year [ 5 ], “develop logical thinking” as part of studies in second year, which was a statement drafted by the lead author in fact [ 6 ], and “be fluent in analysing and constructing logical arguments” as part of studies in third year [ 7 ]. The University of Cambridge’s Faculty of Mathematics, UK, provides a dedicated document “Transferable Skills in the Mathematical Tripos” as part of its undergraduate mathematics course information, which again lists “analytic ability; creativity; initiative; logical and methodical reasoning; persistence” [ 8 ].

In contrast, psychological research, which has been empirically investigating the concept of transferability of skills since the early 1900s, points quite oppositely to reasoning skills as being highly domain specific [ 9 ]. Therefore, support for claims that studying mathematics engenders more than specific mathematics knowledge is highly pertinent. And yet it is largely absent. The 2014 Centre for Curriculum Redesign (CCR) four part paper “Mathematics for the 21st Century: What Should Students Learn?” concludes in its fourth paper titled “Does mathematics education enhance higher-order thinking skills?” with a call to action “… there is not sufficient evidence to conclude that mathematics enhances higher order cognitive functions. The CCR calls for a much stronger cognitive psychology and neuroscience research base to be developed on the effects of studying mathematics” [ 10 ].

Inglis and Simpson [ 11 ], bringing up this very issue, examined the ability of first-year undergraduate students from a high-ranking UK university mathematics department, on the “Four Cards Problem” thinking task, also known as the Wason Selection Task. It is stated as follows.

Each of the following cards have a letter on one side and a number on the other.

mathematics in problem solving and decision making

Here is a rule: “if a card has a D on one side, then it has a 3 on the other”. Your task is to select all those cards, but only those cards, which you would have to turn over in order to find out whether the rule is true or false. Which cards would you select?

This task involves understanding conditional inference, namely understanding the rule “If P then Q” and with this, deducing the answer as “P and not Q” or “D and 7”. Such logical deduction indeed presents as a good candidate to test for a potential ability of the mathematically trained. This task has also been substantially investigated in the domain of the psychology of reasoning [ 12 p8] revealing across a wide range of publications that only around 10% of the general population reach the correct result. The predominant mistake being to pick “D and 3”; where in the original study by Wason [ 13 ] it is suggested that this was picked by 65% of people. This poor success rate along with a standard mistake has fuelled interest in the task as well as attempts to understand why it occurs. A prevailing theory being the so named matching bias effect; the effect of disproportionately concentrating on items specifically mentioned in the situation, as opposed to reasoning according to logical rules.

Inglis and Simpson’s results isolated mathematically trained individuals with respect to this task. The participants were under time constraint and 13% of the first-year undergraduate mathematics students sampled reached the correct response, compared to 4% of the non-mathematics (arts) students that was included. Of note also was the 24% of mathematics students as opposed to 45% of the non-mathematics students who chose the standard mistake. The study indeed unveiled that mathematically trained individuals were significantly less affected by the matching bias effect with this problem than the individuals without mathematics training. However, the achievement of the mathematically trained group was still far from masterful and the preponderance for a non-standard mistake compared with non-mathematically trained people is suggestive. Mathematical training appears to engender a different thinking style, but it remains unclear what the difference is.

Inglis, Simpson and colleagues proceeded to follow up their results with a number of studies concentrated on conditional inference in general [ 14 , 15 ]. A justification for this single investigatory pathway being that if transfer of knowledge is present, something subtle to test for in the first place, a key consideration should be the generalisation of learning rather than the application of skills learned in one context to another (where experimenter bias in the choice of contexts is more likely to be an issue). For this they typically used sixteen “if P then Q” comprehension tasks, where their samples across a number of studies have included 16-year-old pre-university mathematics students (from England and Cyprus), mathematics honours students in their first year of undergraduate university study, third year university mathematics students, and associated control groups. The studies have encompassed controls for general intelligence and thinking disposition prior to training, as well as follows ups of up to two years to address the issue of causation. The conclusive thinking pattern that has emerged is a tendency of the mathematical groups towards a greater likelihood of rejecting the invalid denial of the antecedent and affirmation of the consequent inferences. But with this, and this was validated by a second separate study, the English mathematics group actually became less likely to endorse the valid modus tollens inference. So again, mathematical training appears to engender a different thinking style, but there are subtleties and it remains unclear what the exact difference is.

This project was designed to broaden the search on the notion that mathematics training leads to increased reasoning skills. We focused on a range of reasoning problems considered in psychological research to be particularly insightful into decision making, critical thinking and logical deduction, with their distinction in that the general population generally struggles with answering them correctly. An Australian sample adds diversity to the current enquiries that have been European focussed. Furthermore, in an effort to identify the impact of mathematics training through a possible gradation effect, different levels of mathematically trained individuals were tested for performance.

Well-studied thinking tasks from a variety of psychological studies were chosen. Their descriptions, associated success rates and other pertinent details follows. They were all chosen as the correct answer is typically eluded for a standard mistake.

The three-item Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) was used as introduced by Frederick [ 16 ]. This test was devised in line with the theory that there are two general types of cognitive activity: one that operates quickly and without reflection, and another that requires not only conscious thought and effort, but also an ability to reflect on one’s own cognition by including a step of suppression of the first to reach it. The three items in the test involve an incorrect “gut” response and further cognitive skill is deemed required to reach the correct answer (although see [ 17 ] for evidence that correct responses can result from “intuition”, which could be related to intelligence [ 18 ]).

In a lake, there is a patch of lily pads. Every day, the patch doubles in size. If it takes 48 days for the patch to cover the entire lake, how long would it take for the patch to cover half of the lake?

If it takes 5 machines 5 minutes to make 5 widgets, how long would it take 100 machines to make 100 widgets?

Bat and ball

A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs a dollar more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?

The solutions are: 47 days for the Lily Pads problem, 5 minutes for the Widgets problem and 5 cents for the Bat and Ball problem. The considered intuitive, but wrong, answers are 24 days, 100 minutes and 10 cents, respectively. These wrong answers are attributed to participants becoming over focused on the numbers so as to ignore the exponential growth pattern in the Lily Pads problem, merely complete a pattern in numbers in the Widgets problem, and neglect the relationship “more than” in the Bat and Ball problem [ 19 ]. The original study by Frederick [ 16 ] provides a composite measure of the performance on these three items, with only 17% of those studied (n = 3428) reaching the perfect score. The CRT has since been studied extensively [ 19 – 21 ]. Research using the CRT tends not to report performance on the individual items of the test, but rather a composite measure of performance. Attridge and Inglis [ 22 ] used the CRT as a test for thinking disposition of mathematics students as one way to attempt to disentangle the issue of filtering according to prior thinking styles rather than transference of knowledge in successful problem solving. They repeat tested 16-year old pre-university mathematics students and English literature students without mathematics subjects at a one-year interval and found no difference between groups.

Three problems were included that test the ability to reason about probability. All three problems were originally discussed by Kahneman and Tversky [ 23 ], with the typically poor performance on these problems explained by participants relying not on probability knowledge, but a short-cut method of thinking known as the representativeness heuristic. In the late 1980s, Richard Nisbett and colleagues showed that graduate level training in statistics, while not revealing any improvement in logical reasoning, did correlate with higher-quality statistical answers [ 24 ]. Their studies lead in particular to the conclusion that comprehension of, what is known as the law of large numbers, did show improvement with training. The first of our next three problems targeted this law directly.

  • (a). the larger hospital
  • (b). the smaller hospital
  • (c). about the same (that is, within 5 percent of each other)

Kahneman and Tversky [ 23 ] reported that, of 50 participants, 12 chose (a), 10 chose (b), and 28 chose (c). The correct answer is (b), for the reason that small samples are more likely to exhibit extreme events than large samples from the same population. The larger the sample, the more likely it will exhibit characteristics of the parent population, such as the proportion of boys to girls. However, people tend to discount or be unaware of this feature of sampling statistics, which Kahneman and Tversky refer to as the law of large numbers. Instead, according to Kahneman and Tversky, people tend to adhere to a fallacious law of small numbers, where even small samples are expected to exhibit properties of the parent population, as illustrated by the proportion of participants choosing the answer (c) in their 1972 study. Such thinking reflects use of the representativeness heuristic, whereby someone will judge the likelihood of an uncertain event based on how similar it is to characteristics of the parent population of events.

Birth order

  • (a). What is your estimate of the number of families surveyed in which the exact order of births was BGBBBB?
  • (b). In the same survey set, which, if any, of the following two sequences would be more likely: BBBGGG or GBBGBG?

All of the events listed in the problem have an equal probability, so the correct answer to (a) is 72, and to (b) is “neither is more likely”. Kahneman and Tversky [ 23 ] reported that 75 of 92 participants judged the sequence in (a) as less likely than the given sequence. A similar number (unspecified by Kahneman and Tversky, but the statistical effect was reported to be of the same order as in (a)) reported that GBBGBG was the more likely sequence. Again, Kahneman and Tversky suggested that these results reflected use of the representativeness heuristic. In the context of this problem, the heuristic would have taken the following form: some birth orders appear less patterned than others, and less patterned is to be associated with the randomness of birth order, making them more likely.

Coin tosses

  • (a). H T H T H T H T
  • (b). H H H H T T T T
  • (c). T T H H T T H H
  • (d). H T T H T H H T
  • (e). all of the above are equally likely

The correct answer in this problem is (e). Kahneman and Tversky [ 23 ] reported that participants tend to choose less patterned looking sequences (e.g., H T T H T H H T) as more likely than more systematic looking sequences (e.g., H T H T H T H T). This reasoning again reflects the representativeness heuristic.

Three further questions from the literature were included to test problem solving skill.

Two drivers

  • (a). Driver A would win the race
  • (b). Driver B would win the race
  • (c). the two drivers would arrive at the same time (within a few seconds of one another)

This problem was developed by Pelham and Neter [ 25 ]. The correct answer is (a), which can be determined by calculations of driving times for each Driver, using time = distance/velocity. Pelham and Neter argue, however, that (c) is intuitively appealing, on the basis that both drivers appear to have the same overall average speed. Pelham and Neter reported that 67% of their sample gave this incorrect response to the problem, and a further 13% selected (b).

Petrol station

Imagine that you are driving along the road and you notice that your car is running low on petrol. You see two petrol stations next to each other, both advertising their petrol prices. Station A’s price is 65c/litre; Station B’s price is 60c/litre. Station A’s sign also announces: “5c/litre discount for cash!” Station B’s sign announces “5c/litre surcharge for credit cards.” All other factors being equal (for example, cleanliness of the stations, number of cars waiting at each etc), to which station would you choose to go, and why?

This problem was adapted from one described by Galotti [ 26 ], and is inspired by research reported by Thaler [ 27 ]. According to Thaler’s research, most people prefer Station A, even though both stations are offering the same deal: 60c/litre for cash, and 65c/litre for credit. Tversky and Kahneman [ 28 ] explain this preference by invoking the concept of framing effects. In the context of this problem, such an effect would involve viewing the outcomes as changes from some initial point. The initial point frames the problem, and provides a context for viewing the outcome. Thus, depending on the starting point, outcomes in this problem can be viewed as either a gain (in Station A, you gain a discount if you use cash) or a loss (in Station B, you are charged more (a loss) for using credit). Given that people are apparently more concerned about a loss than a gain [ 29 ], the loss associated with Station B makes it the less attractive option, and hence the preference for Station A. The correct answer, though, is that the stations are offering the same deal and so no station should be preferred.

And finally, a question described by Stanovich [ 30 , 31 ] as testing our predisposition for cognitive operations that require the least computational effort.

Jack looking at Anne

  • (c). Cannot be determined

Stanovich reported that over 80% of people choose the “lazy” answer (c). The correct answer is (a).

The above questions survey, in a clear problem solving setting, an ability to engage advanced cognitive processing in order to critically evaluate and possibly override initial gut reasoning, an ability to reason about probability within the framework of the law of large numbers and the relationship between randomness and patterning, an ability to isolate salient features of a problem and, with the last question in particular, an ability to map logical relations. It might be hypothesised that according to degrees of mathematical training, in line with the knowledge base provided and the claims of associated broad and enhanced problem-solving abilities in general, that participants with greater degrees of such training would outperform others on these questions. This hypothesis was investigated in this study. In addition, given that no previous study on this issue has examined the variety of problems used in this study, we also undertook an exploratory analysis to investigate whether there exist any associations between the problems in terms of their likelihood of correct solution. Similarities between problems might indicate which problem solving domains could be susceptible to the effects of mathematics training.

  • Introductory—First year, second semester, university students with weak high school mathematical results, only enrolled in the current unit as a compulsory component for their chosen degree, a unit not enabling any future mathematical pathway, a typical student may be enrolled in a Biology or Geography major;
  • Standard—First year, second semester, university students with fair to good high school mathematical results, enrolled in the current mathematics unit as a compulsory component for their chosen degree with the possibility of including some further mathematical units in their degree pathway, a typical student may be enrolled in an IT or Computer Science major;
  • Advanced1—First year, second semester, university mathematics students with very strong interest as well as background in mathematics, all higher year mathematical units are included as possible future pathway, a typical student may be enrolled in a Mathematics or Physics major;
  • Advanced2—Second year, second semester, university mathematics students with strong interest as well as background in mathematics, typically a direct follow on from the previously mentioned Advanced1 cohort;
  • Academic—Research academics in the mathematical sciences.

Participants

123 first year university students volunteered during “help on demand” tutorial times containing up to 30 students. These are course allocated times that are supervised yet self-directed by students. This minimised disruption and discouraged coercion. 44 second year university students completed the questionnaire during a weekly one-hour time slot dedicated to putting the latest mathematical concepts to practice with the lecturer (whereby contrast to what occurs in tutorial times the lecturer does most of the work and all students enrolled are invited). All these university students completed the questionnaire in normal classroom conditions; they were not placed under strict examination conditions. The lead author walked around to prevent discussion and coercion and there was minimum disruption. 30 research academics responded to local advertising and answered the questionnaire in their workplace while supervised.

The questionnaires were voluntary, anonymous and confidential. Participants were free to withdraw from the study at any time and without any penalty. No participant took this option however. The questionnaires gathered demographic information which included age, level of education attained and current qualification pursued, name of last qualification and years since obtaining it, and an option to note current speciality for research academics. Each problem task was placed on a separate page. Participants were not placed under time constraint, but while supervised, were asked to write their start and finish times on the front page of the survey to note approximate completion times. Speed of completion was not incentivised. Participants were not allowed to use calculators. A final “Comments Page” gave the option for feedback including specifically if the participants had previously seen any of the questions. Questionnaires were administered in person and supervised to avoid collusion or consulting of external sources.

The responses were coded four ways: A) correct; B) standard error (the errors discussed above in The Study); C) other error; D) left blank.

The ethical aspects of the study were approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Sydney, protocol number [2016/647].

The first analysis examined the total number of correct responses provided by the participants as a function of group. Scores ranged from 1 to 11 out of a total possible of 11 (Problem 6 had 2 parts) ( Fig 1 ). An ANOVA of this data indicated a significant effect of group (F(4, 192) = 20.426, p < .001, partial η 2 = .299). Pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s HSD test indicated that the Introductory group performed significantly worse than the Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups. There were no significant differences between the Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups.

thumbnail

  • PPT PowerPoint slide
  • PNG larger image
  • TIFF original image

Error bars are one standard error of the mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153.g001

Overall solution time, while recorded manually and approximately, was positively correlated with group, such that the more training someone had received, the longer were these solution times (r(180) = 0.247, p = .001). However, as can be seen in Fig 2 , this relationship is not strong.

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153.g002

A series of chi-squared analyses, and their Bayesian equivalents, were performed on each problem, to determine whether the distribution of response types differed as a function of group. To minimise the number of cells in which expected values in some of these analyses were less than 5, the Standard Error, Other Error and Blank response categories were collapsed into one category (Incorrect Response). For three of the questions, the expected values of some cells did fall below 5, and this was due to most people getting the problem wrong (Four Cards), or most people correctly responding to the problem (Bat and Ball, Coin Tosses). In these cases, the pattern of results was so clear that a statistical analysis was barely required. Significant chi-squared results were examined further with pairwise posthoc comparisons (see Table 1 ).

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153.t001

The three groups with the least amount of training in mathematics were far less likely than the other groups to give the correct solution (χ 2 (4) = 31.06, p < .001; BF 10 = 45,045) ( Table 1 ). People in the two most advanced groups (Advanced2 and Academic) were more likely to solve the card problem correctly, although it was still less than half of the people in these groups who did so. Further, these people were less likely to give the standard incorrect solution, so that most who were incorrect suggested some more cognitively elaborate answer, such as turning over all cards. The proportion of people in the Advanced2 and Academic groups (39 and 37%) who solved the problem correctly far exceeded the typical proportion observed with this problem (10%). Of note, also, is the relatively high proportion of those in the higher training groups who, when they made an error, did not make the standard error, a similar result to the one reported by Inglis and Simpson [ 11 ].

The cognitive reflection test

In the Lily Pads problem, although most people in the Standard, Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups were likely to select the correct solution, it was also the case that the less training someone had received in mathematics, the more likely they were to select an incorrect solution (χ 2 (4) = 27.28, p < .001; BF 10 = 15,554), with the standard incorrect answer being the next most prevalent response for the two lower ability mathematics groups ( Table 1 ).

Performance on the Widgets problem was similar to performance on the Lily Pads problem in that most people in the Standard, Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups were likely to select the correct solution, but that the less training someone had received in mathematics, the more likely they were to select an incorrect solution (χ 2 (4) = 23.76, p< .001; BF 10 = 516) ( Table 1 ). As with the Lily Pads and Widget problems, people in the Standard, Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups were highly likely to solve the Bat and Ball problem (χ 2 (4) = 35.37, p < .001; BF 10 = 208,667). Errors were more likely from the least mathematically trained people (Introductory, Standard) than the other groups ( Table 1 ).

To compare performance on the CRT with previously published results, performance on the three problems (Lily Pads, Widgets, Bat and Ball) were combined. The number of people in each condition that solved 0, 1, 2, or 3 problems correctly is presented in Table 2 . The Introductory group were evenly distributed amongst the four categories, with 26% solving all three problems correctly. Around 70% of the rest of the groups solved all 3 problems correctly, which is vastly superior to the 17% reported by Frederick [ 16 ].

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153.t002

Responses to the Hospitals problem were almost universally split between correct and standard errors in the Standard, Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups. Although this pattern of responses was also evident in the Introductory group, this group also exhibited more non-standard errors and non-responses than the other groups. However, the differences between the groups were not significant (χ 2 (4) = 4.93, p = .295; BF 10 = .068) ( Table 1 ). Nonetheless, the performance of all groups exceeds the 20% correct response rate reported by Kahneman and Tversky [ 23 ].

The two versions of the Birth Order problem showed similar results, with correct responses being more likely in the groups with more training (i.e., Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic), and responses being shared amongst the various categories in the Introductory and Standard groups (χ a 2 (4) = 24.54, p < .001; BF 10 = 1,303; χ b 2 (4) = 25.77, p < .001; BF 10 = 2,970) ( Table 1 ). Nonetheless, performance on both versions of the problem in this study was significantly better than the 82% error rate reported by Kahneman and Tversky [ 23 ].

The Coin Tosses problem was performed well by all groups, with very few people in any condition committing errors. There were no obvious differences between the groups (χ 2 (4) = 3.70, p = .448; BF 10 = .160) ( Table 1 ). Kahneman and Tversky [ 23 ] reported that people tend to make errors on this type of problem by choosing less patterned looking sequences, but they did not report relative proportions of people making errors versus giving correct responses. Clearly the sample in this study did not perform like those in Kahneman and Tversky’s study.

Responses on the Two Drivers problem were clearly distinguished by a high chance of error in the Introductory and Standard groups (over 80%), and a fairly good chance of being correct in the Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups (χ 2 (4) = 46.16, p < .001; BF 10 = 1.32 x 10 8 ) ( Table 1 ). Academics were the standout performers on this problem, although over a quarter of this group produced an incorrect response. Thus, the first two groups performed similarly to the participants in the Pelham and Neter [ 25 ] study, 80% of whom gave an incorrect response.

Responses on the Petrol Station problem were marked by good performance by the Academic group (73% providing a correct response), and just over half of each of the other groups correctly solving the problem. This difference was not significant (χ 2 (4) = 4.68, p = .322: BF 10 = .059) ( Table 1 ). Errors were fairly evenly balanced between standard and other, except for the Academic group, who were more likely to provide a creative answer if they made an error. Thaler [ 27 ] reported that most people get this problem wrong. In this study, however, on average, most people got this problem correct, although this average was boosted by the Academic group.

Responses on the Jack looking at Anne problem generally were standard errors, except for the Advanced2 and Academic groups, which were evenly split between standard errors and correct responses (χ 2 (4) = 18.03, p = .001; BF 10 = 46) ( Table 1 ). Thus, apart from these two groups, the error rate in this study was similar to that reported by Stanovich [ 30 ], where 80% of participants were incorrect.

A series of logistic regression analyses were performed in order to examine whether the likelihood of solving a particular problem correctly could be predicted on the basis of whether other problems were solved correctly. Each analysis involved selecting performance (correct or error) on one problem as the outcome variable, and performance on the other problems as predictor variables. Training (amount of training) was also included as a predictor variable in each analysis. A further logistic regression was performed with training as the outcome variable, and performance on all of the problems as predictor variables. The results of these analyses are summarised in Table 3 . There were three multi-variable relationships observed in these analyses, which can be interpreted as the likelihood of solving one problem in each group being associated with solving the others in the set. These sets were: (1) Lily Pads, Widgets and Petrol Station; (2) Hospitals, Four Cards and Two Drivers; (3) Birth Order and Coin Tosses. Training also featured in each of these sets, moderating the relationships as per the results presented above for each problem.

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153.t003

The final “Comments Page” revealed the participants as overwhelmingly enjoying the questions. Any analysis of previous exposure to the tasks proved impossible as there was little to no alignment on participant’s degree of recall, if any, and even perceptions of what exposure entailed. For example, some participants confused being exposed to the particular tasks with being habitually exposed to puzzles, or even mathematics problems, more broadly.

In general, the amount of mathematics training a group had received predicted their performance on the overall set of problems. The greater the training, the more problems were answered correctly, and the slower the recorded response times. There was not an obvious difference between the Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups on either of these measures, however there were clear differences between this group and the Introductory and Standard groups, with the former exhibiting clearly superior accuracy. While time records were taken approximately, so as to avoid adding time pressure as a variable, that the Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups recorded more time in their consideration of the problems, may suggest a “pause and consider” approach to such problems is a characteristic of the advanced groups. This is in line with what was suggested by an eye-movement tracking study of mathematically trained students attempting the Four Cards Problem; where participants that had not chosen the standard error had spent longer considering the card linked to the matching bias effect [ 14 ]. It is important to note, however, that longer response times may reflect other cognitive processes than deliberation [ 32 ].

Performance on some problems was associated with performance on other problems. That is, if someone correctly answered a problem in one of these sets, they were also highly likely to correctly answer the other problems in the set. These sets were: (1) Lily Pads, Widgets and Petrol Station; (2) Hospitals, Four Cards and Two Drivers; (3) Birth Order and Coin Tosses. This is different with how these problems have been typically clustered a priori in the research literature: (I) Lily Pads, Widgets and Bat and Ball (CRT); (II) Hospitals and Two Drivers (explained below); (III) Hospitals, Birth Order and Coin Tosses (representativeness heuristic); (IV) Birth Order and Coin Tosses (probability theory). Consideration of these problem groupings follows.

Correctly answering all three problems in (I) entailed not being distracted by particular pieces of information in the problems so as to stay focused on uncovering the real underlying relationships. The Lily Pads and Widget problems can mislead if attention is over focused on the numbers, and conversely, the Petrol Station problem can mislead if there is too much focus on the idea of a discount. While the Lily Pads and Widget problems are traditionally paired with the Bat and Ball problem in the CRT, it may be that performance on the Bat and Ball problem did not appear as part of this set due to an added level of difficulty. With the problems in (I), avoiding being distracted by certain parts of the questions at the expense of others almost leads directly to the correct answer. However, with the Bat and Ball problem, further steps in mathematical reasoning still need to occur in answering which two numbers add together to give a result while also subtracting one from the other for another.

With the problems in (II) it is of interest that the Two Drivers problem was created specifically to be paired with the Hospitals problem to test for motivation in problem solving [ 23 ]. Within this framework further transparent versions of these problems were successfully devised to manipulate for difficulty. The Two Drivers problem was amended to have Driver B travelling at exactly 5 mph during the first half of the race and at exactly 95 mph during the last half of the race. The Hospitals problem was amended so the smaller hospital would have “only 2” babies born each day and where for a period of one year the hospitals recorded the number of days on which all of the babies born were boys. Could the association in (II) be pointing to how participants overcome initial fictitious mathematical rules? Maybe they reframe the question in simpler terms to see the pattern. The Four Cards Problem also elicited a high number of incorrect answers where, associated with mathematical training, the standard incorrect solution was avoided for more cognitively elaborate ones. Indeed, a gradation effect appeared across the groups where the standard error of the “D and 3” cards becomes “D only” ( Table 4 ). Adrian Simpson and Derrick Watson found a comparable result across their two groups [14 p61]. This could again be pointing to having avoided an initial fictitious rule of simply concentrating on items directly found in the question, participants then seek to reframe the question to unearth the logical rule to be deduced. An added level of difficulty with this question may be why participants become trapped in a false answer. The eye-movement tracking study mentioned above supports this theory.

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153.t004

The problems in (III) fit naturally together as part of basic probability theory, a topic participants would have assimilated, or not, as part of various education curricula. While the equal likelihood of all possible outcomes with respect to a coin toss may be culturally assimilated, the same may not be as straightforward for birth gender outcomes where such assumptions could be swayed by biological hypothesis or folk wisdom [ 33 ]. The gradation of the results in terms of mathematical training does not support this possibility.

The effect of training on performance accuracy was more obvious in some problems compared to others, and to some extent, this was related to the type of problem. For instance, most of the problems in which performance was related to training (Four Cards, CRT [Lily Pads, Widgets, Bat and Ball], Two Drivers, Jack looking at Anne) could be classed as relying on logical and/or critical thinking. The one exception was the Birth Order problems, which are probability related.

In contrast, two of the three problems in which training did not appear to have much impact on performance (Hospitals and Coin Tosses) require domain-specific knowledge. The Hospitals problem requires a degree of knowledge about sampling statistics. This is a topic of quite distinct flavour that not all mathematically trained individuals gain familiarity with. On the other hand, all groups having performed well on the Coin Tosses problem is in line with a level of familiarity with basic probability having been originally presented at high school. While the questioning of patterning as negatively correlated with randomness is similar to that appearing in the Birth Order question, in the Birth Order question this aspect is arguably more concealed. These results and problem grouping (III) could be pointing to an area for improvement in teaching where the small gap in knowledge required to go from answering the Coin Tosses problem correctly to achieving similarly with the Birth Order problem could be easily addressed. A more formal introduction to sampling statistics in mathematical training could potentially bridge this gap as well as further be extended towards improvement on the Hospitals problem.

The other problem where performance was unrelated to training, the Petrol Station problem, cannot be characterised similarly. It is more of a logical/critical thinking type problem, where there remains some suggestion that training may have impacted performance, as the Academic group seemed to perform better than the rest of the sample. An alternate interpretation of this result is therefore that this problem should not be isolated but grouped with the other problems where performance is affected by training.

  • The Introductory group’s mathematics high school syllabus studied prior to first semester course entry covered: Functions, Trigonometric Functions, Calculus (Introduction to Differentiation, Applications of the Derivative, Antiderivatives, Areas and the Definite Integral), Financial Mathematics, Statistical Analysis. The Introductory group then explored concepts in mathematical modelling with emphasis on the importance of calculus in their first semester of mathematical studies.
  • The Standard group’s mathematics high school syllabus studied prior to first semester course entry covered: Functions, Trigonometric Functions, Calculus (Rates of Change, Integration including the method of substitution, trigonometric identities and inverse trigonometric functions, Areas and Volumes of solids of revolution, some differential equations), Combinatorics, Proof (with particular focus on Proof by Mathematical Induction), Vectors (with application to projectile motion), Statistical Analysis. In first semester their mathematical studies then covered a number of topics the Advanced1 group studied prior to gaining entrance at university; further details on this are given below.
  • The Advanced1 group’s mathematics high school syllabus studied prior to first semester course entry covered: the same course content the Standard group covered at high school plus extra topics on Proof (develop rigorous mathematical arguments and proofs, specifically in the context of number and algebra and further develop Proof by Mathematical Induction), Vectors (3 dimensional vectors, vector equations of lines), Complex Numbers, Calculus (Further Integration techniques with partial fractions and integration by parts), Mechanics (Application of Calculus to Mechanics with simple harmonic motion, modelling motion without and with resistance, projectiles and resisted motion). The Standard group cover these topics in their first semester university studies in mathematics with the exclusion of further concepts of Proof or Mechanics. In first semester the Advanced1 group have built on their knowledge with an emphasis on both theoretical and foundational aspects, as well as developing the skill of applying mathematical theory to solve practical problems. Theoretical topics include a host of theorems relevant to the study of Calculus.

In summary, at the point of our study, the Advanced1 group had more knowledge and practice on rigorous mathematical arguments and proofs in the context of number and algebra, and more in-depth experience with Proofs by Induction, but the bulk of extra knowledge rests with a much deeper knowledge of Calculus. They have had longer experience with a variety of integration techniques, and have worked with a variety of applications of calculus to solve practical problems, including a large section on mechanics at high school. In first semester at university there has been a greater focus on theoretical topics including a host of theorems and associated proofs relevant to the topics studied. As compared to the Introductory and Standard groups, the Advanced1 group have only widened the mathematics knowledge gap since their choice of post-compulsory mathematics at high school. The Advanced2 group come directly from an Advanced1 cohort. And the Academics group would have reached the Advanced1 group’s proficiency as part of their employment. So, are specific reasoning skills resulting from this level of abstract reasoning? Our findings suggest this should certainly be an area of investigation and links in interestingly with other research work. In studying one of the thinking tasks in particular (the Four Cards Problem) and its context of conditional inference more specifically, Inglis and Simpson [ 15 ] found a clear difference between undergraduates in mathematics and undergraduates in other university disciplines, yet also showed a lack of development over first-year university studies on conditional inference measures. A follow up study by Attridge and Inglis [ 22 ] then zeroed in on post-compulsory high school mathematical training and found that students with such training did develop their conditional reasoning to a greater extent than their control group over the course of a year, despite them having received no explicit tuition in conditional logic. The development though, whilst demonstrated as not being the result of a domain-general change in cognitive capacity or thinking disposition, and most likely associated with the domain-specific study of mathematics, revealed a complex pattern of endorsing more of some inferences and less of others. The study here focused on a much broader problem set associated with logical and critical thinking and it too is suggestive of a more complex picture in how mathematics training may be contributing to problem solving styles. A more intricate pattern to do with the impact of mathematical training on problem solving techniques is appearing as required for consideration.

There is also a final interpretation to consider: that people in the Advanced 1, Advanced2 and Academic groups did not gain anything from their mathematics training in terms of their ability to solve these problems. Instead, with studies denying any correlation of many of these problems with what is currently measured as intelligence [ 30 ], they might still be people of a particular intelligence or thinking disposition to start with, who have been able to use that intelligence to not only solve these problems, but also survive the challenges of their mathematics training.

That the CRT has been traditionally used as a measure of baseline thinking disposition and that performance has been found to be immutable across groups tested is of particular interest since our results show a clear possible training effect on these questions. CRT is tied with a willingness to engage in effortful thinking which presents as a suitable ability for training. It is beyond the scope of this study, but a thorough review of CRT testing is suggestive of a broader appreciation and better framework to understand thinking disposition, ability and potential ability.

Mathematical training appears associated with certain thinking skills, but there are clearly some subtleties that need to be extricated. The thinking tasks here add to the foundational results where the aim is for a firmer platform on which to eventually base more targeted and illustrative inquiry. If thinking skills can be fostered, could first year university mathematics teaching be improved so that all samples from that group reach the Advanced1 group level of reasoning? Do university mathematics courses become purely about domain-specific knowledge from this point on? Intensive training has been shown to impact the brain and cognition across a number of domains from music [ 34 ], to video gaming [ 35 ], to Braille reading [ 36 ]. The hypothesis that mathematics, with its highly specific practice, fits within this list remains legitimate, but simply unchartered. With our current level of understanding it is worth appreciating the careful wording of the NYU Courant Institute on ‘Why Study Math?’ where there is no assumption of causation: “Mathematicians need to have good reasoning ability in order to identify, analyze, and apply basic logical principles to technical problems.” [ 37 ].

Limitations

One possible limitation of the current study is that the problems may have been too easy for the more advanced people, and so we observed a ceiling effect (i.e., some people obtained 100% correct on all problems). This was most obvious in the Advanced1, Advanced2 and Academic groups. It is possible that participants in these groups had developed logical and critical thinking skills throughout their mathematical training that were sufficient to cope with most of the problems used in this study, and so this would support the contention that training in mathematics leads to the development of logical and critical thinking skills useful in a range of domains. Another interpretation is that participants in these groups already possessed the necessary thinking skills for solving the problems in this study, which is why they are able to cope with the material in the advanced units they were enrolled in, or complete a PhD in mathematics and hold down an academic position in a mathematics department. This would then suggest that training in mathematics had no effect on abstract thinking skills—people in this study possessed them to varying extents prior to their studies. This issue might be settled in a future study that used a greater number of problems of varying difficulties to maximise the chances of finding a difference between the three groups with the most amount of training. Alternatively, a longitudinal study that followed people through their mathematics training could determine whether their logical and critical thinking abilities changed throughout their course.

A further limitation of the study may be that several of the reasoning biases examined in this study were measured by only one problem each (i.e., Four Cards Problem, Two Drivers, Petrol Station, Jack looking at Anne). A more reliable measure of these biases could be achieved by including more problems that tap into these biases. This would, however, increase the time required of participants during data collection, and in the context of this study, would mean a different mode of testing would likely be required.

Broad sweeping intuitive claims of the transferable skills endowed by a study of mathematics require evidence. Our study uniquely covers a wide range of participants, from limited mathematics training through to research academics in the mathematical sciences. It furthermore considered performance on 11 well-studied thinking tasks that typically elude participants in psychological studies and on which results have been uncorrelated with general intelligence, education levels and other demographic information [ 15 , 16 , 30 ]. We identified different performances on these tasks with respect to different groups, based on level of mathematical training. This included the CRT which has developed into a method of measuring baseline thinking disposition. We identified different distributions of types of errors for the mathematically trained. We furthermore identified a performance threshold that exists in first year university for those with high level mathematics training. This study then provides insight into possible changes and adjustments to mathematics courses in order for them to fulfil their advertised goal of reaching improved rational and logical reasoning for a higher number of students.

It is central to any education program to have a clear grasp of the nature of what it delivers and how, but arguably especially so for the core discipline that is mathematics. In 2014 the Office of The Chief Scientist of Australia released a report “Australia’s STEM workforce: a survey of employers” where transferable skills attributed to mathematics were also ones that employers deemed as part of the most valuable [ 38 ]. A better understanding of what mathematics delivers in this space is an opportunity to truly capitalise on this historical culture-crossing subject.

Supporting information

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236153.s001

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Jacqui Ramagge for her proof reading and input, as well as support towards data collection.

  • 1. Smith A. Making mathematics count: The report of Professor Adrian Smith’s inquiry into post-14 mathematics education. 2004. London: The Stationery Office.
  • 2. AMSI, Vision for a Maths Nation. 2015. http://amsi.org.au/publications/a-vision-for-a-maths-nation/
  • 3. British Columbia [Internet]. Mathematics; Goals and Rationale. 2016 [cited 2019 Dec 5]. https://curriculum.gov.bc.ca/curriculum/mathematics/core/goals-and-rationale
  • 4. Monash University [Internet]. Mathematical Sciences. 2019 [cited 2019 Jul 30]. https://www.monash.edu/science/schools/mathematical-sciences/current .
  • 5. The University of Sydney [Internet]. MATH1014. 2017 [cited 2019 Dec 5]. http://www.maths.usyd.edu.au/u/UG/TU/YR1ADMIN/r/MATH1014.pdf .
  • 6. The University of Sydney [Internet]. MATH2965. 2016 [cited 2016 Dec 12]. http://www.maths.usyd.edu.au/u/UG/IM/MATH2965/
  • 7. The University of Sydney [Internet]. MATH3066. 2017 [cited 2017 Dec 8]. http://www.maths.usyd.edu.au/u/UG/SM/MATH3066/r/2017info3066.pdf .
  • 8. Cambridge University [Internet]. Mathematical Tripos. 2019 [cited 2019 Jul 30]. https://www.maths.cam.ac.uk/undergrad/course/transferable_skills .
  • 9. Speelman CP, Kirsner K. Beyond the learning curve: The construction of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.
  • 10. Fadel C. Mathematics for the 21 st Century: What Should Students Learn? Boston, Massachusetts: Center for Curriculum Redesign; 2014.
  • 11. Inglis M, Simpson A. Heuristic biases in mathematical reasoning. In: Chick HL, Vincent JL, editors. Proceedings of the 29th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. Melbourne: PME; 2005. p. 177–84.
  • 12. Manktelow KI. Reasoning and Thinking. UK: Psychology Press; 1999.
  • View Article
  • PubMed/NCBI
  • Google Scholar
  • 14. Inglis M, Attridge N. Does mathematical study develop logical thinking? Testing the theory of formal discipline. London: World Scientific Publishing Europe Ltd; 2016.
  • 24. Nisbett RE. Can reasoning be taught? In: Callan E, Grotzer T, Kagan J, Nisbett RE, Perkins DN, Shulman LS, editors. Education and a Civic Society: Teaching Evidence-based Decision Making. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of Arts & Sciences; 2009.
  • 26. Galotti KM. Cognitive psychology in and out of the laboratory. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole; 1994.
  • 37. NYU [Internet]. Why Study Math? 2019 [cited 2019 Jul 30]. https://math.nyu.edu/dynamic/undergrad/overview/why-study-math/
  • 38. Office of The Chief Scientist. Australia’s STEM workforce: a survey of employers. Barton ACT: Deloitte Access Economics; 2014.

Wonder Math

How to Improve Problem-Solving Skills: Mathematics and Critical Thinking

how-to-improve-problem-solving-skills

In today’s rapidly changing world, problem-solving has become a quintessential skill. When we discuss the topic, it’s natural to ask, “What is problem-solving?” and “How can we enhance this skill, particularly in children?” The discipline of mathematics offers a rich platform to explore these questions. Through math, not only do we delve into numbers and equations, but we also explore how to improve problem-solving skills and how to develop critical thinking skills in math. Let’s embark on this enlightening journey together.

What is Problem-Solving?

At its core, problem-solving involves identifying a challenge and finding a solution. But it’s not always as straightforward as it sounds. So, what is problem-solving? True problem-solving requires a combination of creative thinking and logical reasoning. Mathematics, in many ways, embodies this blend. When a student approaches a math problem, they must discern the issue at hand, consider various methods to tackle it, and then systematically execute their chosen strategy.

But what is problem-solving in a broader context? It’s a life skill. Whether we’re deciding the best route to a destination, determining how to save for a big purchase, or even figuring out how to fix a broken appliance, we’re using problem-solving.

How to Develop Critical Thinking Skills in Math

Critical thinking goes hand in hand with problem-solving. But exactly how to develop critical thinking skills in math might not be immediately obvious. Here are a few strategies:

  • Contextual Learning: Teaching math within a story or real-life scenario makes it relevant. When students see math as a tool to navigate the world around them, they naturally begin to think critically about solutions.
  • Open-ended Questions: Instead of merely seeking the “right” answer, encourage students to explain their thought processes. This nudges them to think deeply about their approach.
  • Group Discussions: Collaborative learning can foster different perspectives, prompting students to consider multiple ways to solve a problem.
  • Challenging Problems: Occasionally introducing problems that are a bit beyond a student’s current skill level can stimulate critical thinking. They will have to stretch their understanding and think outside the box.

What are the Six Basic Steps of the Problem-Solving Process?

Understanding how to improve problem-solving skills often comes down to familiarizing oneself with the systematic approach to challenges. So, what are the six basic steps of the problem-solving process?

  • Identification: Recognize and define the problem.
  • Analysis: Understand the problem’s intricacies and nuances.
  • Generation of Alternatives: Think of different ways to approach the challenge.
  • Decision Making: Choose the most suitable method to address the problem.
  • Implementation: Put the chosen solution into action.
  • Evaluation: Reflect on the solution’s effectiveness and learn from the outcome.

By embedding these steps into mathematical education, we provide students with a structured framework. When they wonder about how to improve problem-solving skills or how to develop critical thinking skills in math, they can revert to this process, refining their approach with each new challenge.

Making Math Fun and Relevant

At Wonder Math, we believe that the key to developing robust problem-solving skills lies in making math enjoyable and pertinent. When students see math not just as numbers on a page but as a captivating story or a real-world problem to be solved, their engagement skyrockets. And with heightened engagement comes enhanced understanding.

As educators and parents, it’s crucial to continuously ask ourselves: how can we demonstrate to our children what problem-solving is? How can we best teach them how to develop critical thinking skills in math? And how can we instill in them an understanding of the six basic steps of the problem-solving process?

The answer, we believe, lies in active learning, contextual teaching, and a genuine passion for the beauty of mathematics.

The Underlying Beauty of Mathematics

Often, people perceive mathematics as a rigid discipline confined to numbers and formulas. However, this is a limited view. Math, in essence, is a language that describes patterns, relationships, and structures. It’s a medium through which we can communicate complex ideas, describe our universe, and solve intricate problems. Understanding this deeper beauty of math can further emphasize how to develop critical thinking skills in math.

Why Mathematics is the Ideal Playground for Problem-Solving

Math provides endless opportunities for problem-solving. From basic arithmetic puzzles to advanced calculus challenges, every math problem offers a chance to hone our problem-solving skills. But why is mathematics so effective in this regard?

  • Structured Challenges: Mathematics presents problems in a structured manner, allowing learners to systematically break them down. This format mimics real-world scenarios where understanding the structure of a challenge can be half the battle.
  • Multiple Approaches: Most math problems can be approached in various ways . This teaches learners flexibility in thinking and the ability to view a single issue from multiple angles.
  • Immediate Feedback: Unlike many real-world problems where solutions might take time to show results, in math, students often get immediate feedback. They can quickly gauge if their approach works or if they need to rethink their strategy.

Enhancing the Learning Environment

To genuinely harness the power of mathematics in developing problem-solving skills, the learning environment plays a crucial role. A student who is afraid of making mistakes will hesitate to try out different approaches, stunting their critical thinking growth.

However, in a nurturing, supportive environment where mistakes are seen as learning opportunities, students thrive. They become more willing to take risks, try unconventional solutions, and learn from missteps. This mindset, where failure is not feared but embraced as a part of the learning journey, is pivotal for developing robust problem-solving skills.

Incorporating Technology

In our digital age, technology offers innovative ways to explore math. Interactive apps and online platforms can provide dynamic problem-solving scenarios, making the process even more engaging. These tools can simulate real-world challenges, allowing students to apply their math skills in diverse contexts, further answering the question of how to improve problem-solving skills.

More than Numbers 

In summary, mathematics is more than just numbers and formulas—it’s a world filled with challenges, patterns, and beauty. By understanding its depth and leveraging its structured nature, we can provide learners with the perfect platform to develop critical thinking and problem-solving skills. The key lies in blending traditional techniques with modern tools, creating a holistic learning environment that fosters growth, curiosity, and a lifelong love for learning.

Join us on this transformative journey at Wonder Math. Let’s make math an adventure, teaching our children not just numbers and equations, but also how to improve problem-solving skills and navigate the world with confidence. Enroll your child today and witness the magic of mathematics unfold before your eyes!

FAQ: Mathematics and Critical Thinking

1. what is problem-solving in the context of mathematics.

Problem-solving in mathematics refers to the process of identifying a mathematical challenge and systematically working through methods and strategies to find a solution.

2. Why is math considered a good avenue for developing problem-solving skills?

Mathematics provides structured challenges and allows for multiple approaches to find solutions. This promotes flexibility in thinking and encourages learners to view problems from various angles.

3. How does contextual learning enhance problem-solving abilities?

By teaching math within a story or real-life scenario, it becomes more relevant for the learner. This helps them see math as a tool to navigate real-world challenges , thereby promoting critical thinking.

4. What are the six basic steps of the problem-solving process in math?

The six steps are: Identification, Analysis, Generation of Alternatives, Decision Making, Implementation, and Evaluation.

5. How can parents support their children in developing mathematical problem-solving skills?

Parents can provide real-life contexts for math problems , encourage open discussions about different methods, and ensure a supportive environment where mistakes are seen as learning opportunities.

6. Are there any tools or apps that can help in enhancing problem-solving skills in math?

Yes, there are various interactive apps and online platforms designed specifically for math learning. These tools provide dynamic problem-solving scenarios and simulate real-world challenges, making the learning process engaging.

7. How does group discussion foster critical thinking in math?

Group discussions allow students to hear different perspectives and approaches to a problem. This can challenge their own understanding and push them to think about alternative methods.

8. Is it necessary to always follow the six steps of the problem-solving process sequentially?

While the six steps provide a structured approach, real-life problem-solving can sometimes be more fluid. It’s beneficial to know the steps, but adaptability and responsiveness to the situation are also crucial.

9. How does Wonder Math incorporate active learning in teaching mathematics?

Wonder Math integrates mathematics within engaging stories and real-world scenarios, making it fun and relevant. This active learning approach ensures that students are not just passive recipients but active participants in the learning process.

10. What if my child finds a math problem too challenging and becomes demotivated?

It’s essential to create a supportive environment where challenges are seen as growth opportunities. Remind them that every problem is a chance to learn, and it’s okay to seek help or approach it differently.

Related posts

Summer Math Programs: How They Can Prevent Learning Loss in Young Students

Summer Math Programs: How They Can Prevent Learning Loss in Young Students

As summer approaches, parents and educators alike turn their attention to how they can support young learners during the break. Summer is a time for relaxation, fun, and travel, yet it’s also a critical period when learning loss can occur. This phenomenon, often referred to as the “summer slide,” impacts students’ progress, especially in foundational subjects like mathematics. It’s reported…

I

Math Programs 101: What Every Parent Should Know When Looking For A Math Program

  As a parent, you know that a solid foundation in mathematics is crucial for your child’s success, both in school and in life. But with so many math programs and math help services out there, how do you choose the right one? Whether you’re considering Outschool classes, searching for “math tutoring near me,” or exploring tutoring services online, understanding…

America's Education News Source

Copyright 2024 The 74 Media, Inc

  • Cyberattack
  • absenteeism
  • Future of High School
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • science of reading

Roicki: Math Can Help Teach Students to Make Good Decisions, Inside and Outside the Classroom. Here’s How

mathematics in problem solving and decision making

Untangle Your Mind!

Sign up for our free newsletter and start your day with clear-headed reporting on the latest topics in education.

mathematics in problem solving and decision making

74 Million Reasons to Give

Support The 74’s year-end campaign with a tax-exempt donation and invest in our future.

Most Popular

Why is a grading system touted as more accurate, equitable so hard to implement, in tennessee, the microschooling movement shows no signs of slowing down, americans have yet to accept covid’s tragedy — and are taking it out on schools, on-the-job training prevails as students’ disinterest in college grows, cute: watch a 4th grader explain why thursday is both ‘dress for stem’ & pi day.

Think about the last time you faced a challenging situation. Perhaps you needed to pause and take a moment. You may have weighed how to respond. Once you made your choice, you may have reflected on the result. Was the situation resolved? Left unfinished?

Whether we realize it or not, we constantly make decisions that guide us through our daily lives. Classrooms are no different. Consider the number of daily interactions in a class of 30 students. When students lack good decision-making skills, they can sometimes react to tough situations in negative ways. 

Throughout my 15-year career as a classroom teacher, I have experienced several large classes in which reactive and impulsive interactions between students created a challenging dynamic. A few years into teaching, however, I began to draw students’ attention to their choices and the impact those choices have on themselves and their peers. The result: The overall environment improved. The work wasn’t always easy, but I found effective strategies that can help teachers. Below, I describe them.

Feeling safe

For starters, learning environments have to be safe for good decision-making to take place. In my third-grade classroom, we had regular conversations about ways students could help make our classroom physically safe, such as pushing chairs in and keeping hands and feet under control.

When we talked about intellectual safety, we discussed how the students responded to one another’s ideas. Normalizing errors as part of the learning process and learning to respond constructively helped my students learn to take intellectual risks in class.

Emotional safety included empathizing with one another and considering how actions and words make others feel. 

Decision-making and mathematical problem-solving

Math really is the perfect content area for building good decision-making skills. Solving math problems involves more than simply finding a single correct answer. Students make decisions throughout the process. 

To help develop these skills, teachers should choose tasks that promote analyzing complex situations; identifying givens, constraints, relationships and goals; evaluating their progress; and reflecting on the reasonableness of the solution.

When making sense of problems, students must choose where to enter the problem and plan possible solution pathways. As they work, it’s good practice for the teacher to ask them to select specific tools and defend their choice of those tools. When solving problems, they also need to attend to mathematical relationships in the problem, select appropriate operations to use in their solution and attend to precision in their language, recording and calculations to effectively communicate their reasoning.

Teacher choices affect student choices

When preparing to deliver a math lesson, teachers should try to maximize opportunities for student voice and choice so children develop both strong math skills and good decision-making skills. 

A close analysis of a lesson’s problem set reveals an increase in the complexity of the tasks, as can be seen in the image below. This ladder of complexity serves as the teacher’s road map to designing differentiated pathways from which students can choose during the lesson. The teacher can select different combinations of Must Do, Could Do and Extension problems for independent practice, leading each student toward success with the lesson objective in different ways. 

In the example below, the teacher made the following selections:

mathematics in problem solving and decision making

When designing sets of assignments, teachers should allow students to choose their level of challenge while guiding their decision-making. This means clearly communicating the challenge level of each set and keeping track of student choices. In the example, because each set includes Problem 3 as a Must-Do, all students can work toward the lesson objective and participate in the debrief that follows.

When students overestimate the appropriate level of challenge, a teacher can respond in several ways. For example, when a student is struggling with a selected problem, the teacher might ask what she understands about it, what tools she has tried or whether working with a classmate might help. If she needs more support, the teacher could suggest that she look at a simpler problem to get some ideas for tackling the more challenging one.

Preparing and teaching high-quality lessons is not easy. Teachers must balance content with social and emotional skills to meet a variety of developmental needs. But by incorporating opportunities for choice into math learning, teachers can simultaneously tailor instruction to meet student needs, develop students’ math skills and help students practice decision-making. 

Those are all worthwhile goals that will serve students in and out of math class, as well as outside school.

Joe Roicki is a program specialist on the professional development and implementation success team for Eureka Math, the PK–12 math curriculum from Great Minds. He taught math to students in grades 2-5 students in New York, Florida and Washington state and supported classroom teachers in grades K-8 for 16 years.

Get stories like these delivered straight to your inbox. Sign up for The 74 Newsletter

Joe Roicki is a program specialist on the professional development and implementation success team for Great Minds ’ pre-K-12 Eureka Math curriculum. He taught math to Grades 2-5 students in New York, Florida and Washington states and supported K-8 classroom teachers for 16 years, until the 2017-18 school year.

We want our stories to be shared as widely as possible — for free.

Please view The 74's republishing terms.

By Joe Roicki

mathematics in problem solving and decision making

This story first appeared at The 74 , a nonprofit news site covering education. Sign up for free newsletters from The 74 to get more like this in your inbox.

On The 74 Today

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • v.9(5); 2023 May
  • PMC10208825

Logo of heliyon

Development and differences in mathematical problem-solving skills: A cross-sectional study of differences in demographic backgrounds

Ijtihadi kamilia amalina.

a Doctoral School of Education, University of Szeged, Hungary

Tibor Vidákovich

b Institute of Education, University of Szeged, Hungary

Associated Data

Data will be made available on request.

Problem-solving skills are the most applicable cognitive tool in mathematics, and improving the problem-solving skills of students is a primary aim of education. However, teachers need to know the best period of development and the differences among students to determine the best teaching and learning methods. This study aims to investigate the development and differences in mathematical problem-solving skills of students based on their grades, gender, and school locations. A scenario-based mathematical essay test was administered to 1067 students in grades 7–9 from schools in east Java, Indonesia, and their scores were converted into a logit scale for statistical analysis. The results of a one-way analysis of variance and an independent sample t -test showed that the students had an average level of mathematical problem-solving skills. The number of students who failed increased with the problem-solving phase. The students showed development of problem-solving skills from grade 7 to grade 8 but not in grade 9. A similar pattern of development was observed in the subsample of urban students, both male and female. The demographic background had a significant effect, as students from urban schools outperformed students from rural schools, and female students outperformed male students. The development of problem-solving skills in each phase as well as the effects of the demographic background of the participants were thoroughly examined. Further studies are needed with participants of more varied backgrounds.

1. Introduction

Problem-solving skills are a complex set of cognitive, behavioral, and attitudinal components that are situational and dependent on thorough knowledge and experience [ 1 , 2 ]. Problem-solving skills are acquired over time and are the most widely applicable cognitive tool [ 3 ]. Problem-solving skills are particularly important in mathematics education [ 3 , 4 ]. The development of mathematical problem-solving skills can differ based on age, gender stereotypes, and school locations [ [5] , [6] , [7] , [8] , [9] , [10] ]. Fostering the development of mathematical problem-solving skills is a major goal of educational systems because they provide a tool for success [ 3 , 11 ]. Mathematical problem-solving skills are developed through explicit training and enriching materials [ 12 ]. Teachers must understand how student profiles influence the development of mathematical problem-solving skills to optimize their teaching methods.

Various studies on the development of mathematical problem-solving skills have yielded mixed results. Grissom [ 13 ] concluded that problem-solving skills were fixed and immutable. Meanwhile, other researchers argued that problem-solving skills developed over time and were modifiable, providing an opportunity for their enhancement through targeted educational intervention when problem-solving skills developed quickly [ 3 , 4 , 12 ]. Tracing the development of mathematical problem-solving skills is crucial. Further, the results of previous studies are debatable, necessitating a comprehensive study in the development of students’ mathematical problem-solving skills.

Differences in mathematical problem-solving skills have been identified based on gender and school location [ [6] , [7] , [8] , [9] , [10] ]. School location affects school segregation and school quality [ 9 , 14 ]. The socioeconomic and sociocultural characteristics of a residential area where a school is located are the factors affecting academic achievement [ 14 ]. Studies in several countries have shown that students in urban schools demonstrated better performance and problem-solving skills in mathematics [ 9 , 10 , 15 ]. However, contradictory results have been obtained for other countries [ 6 , 10 ].

Studies on gender differences have shown that male students outperform female students in mathematics, which has piqued the interest of psychologists, sociologists, and educators [ 7 , 16 , 17 ]. The differences appear to be because of brain structure; however, sociologists argue that gender equality can be achieved by providing equal educational opportunities [ 8 , 16 , 18 , 19 ]. Because the results are debatable and no studies on gender differences across grades in schools have been conducted, it would be interesting to investigate gender differences in mathematical problem-solving skills.

Based on the previous explanations, teachers need to understand the best time for students to develop mathematical problem-solving skills because problem-solving is an obligatory mathematics skill to be mastered. However, no relevant studies focused on Indonesia have been conducted regarding the mathematical problem-solving skill development of students in middle school that can provide the necessary information for teachers. Further, middle school is the important first phase of developing critical thinking skills; thus relevant studies are required in this case [ 3 , 4 ]. In addition, a municipal policy-making system can raise differences in problem-solving skills based on different demographic backgrounds [ 10 ]. Moreover, the results of previous studies regarding the development and differences in mathematical problem-solving skills are debatable. Thus, the present study has been conducted to meet these gaps. This study investigated the development of mathematical problem-solving skills in students and the differences owing demographic backgrounds. Three aspects were considered: (1) student profiles of mathematical problem-solving skills, (2) development of their mathematical problem-solving skills across grades, and (3) significant differences in mathematical problem-solving skills based on gender and school location. The results of the present study will provide detailed information regarding the subsample that contributes to the development of mathematical problem-solving skills in students based on their demographic backgrounds. In addition, the description of the score is in the form of a logit scale from large-scale data providing consistent meaning and confident generalization. This study can be used to determine appropriate teaching and learning in the best period of students’ development in mathematical problem-solving skills as well as policies to achieve educational equality.

2. Theoretical background

2.1. mathematical problem-solving skills and their development.

Solving mathematical problems is a complex cognitive ability that requires students to understand the problem as well as apply mathematical concepts to them [ 20 ]. Researchers have described the phases of solving a mathematical problem as understanding the problem, devising a plan, conducting out the plan, and looking back [ [20] , [24] , [21] , [22] , [23] ]. Because mathematical problems are complex, students may struggle with several phases, including applying mathematical knowledge, determining the concepts to use, and stating mathematical sentences (e.g., arithmetic) [ 20 ]. Studies have concluded that more students fail at later stages of the solution process [ 25 , 26 ]. In other words, fewer students fail in the phase of understanding a problem than during the plan implementation phase. Different studies have stated that students face difficulties in understanding the problem, determining what to assume, and investigating relevant information [ 27 ]. This makes them unable to translate the problem into a mathematical form.

Age or grade is viewed as one factor that influences mathematical problem-solving skills because the skills of the students improve over time as a result of the teaching and learning processes [ 28 ]. Neuroscience research has shown that older students have fewer problems with arithmetic than younger students; however, the hemispheric asymmetry is reduced [ 29 ]. In other words, older students are more proficient, but their flexibility to switch among different strategies is less. Ameer & Sigh [ 28 ] obtained similar results and found a considerable difference in mathematical achievement; specifically, older students performed better than younger students in number sense and computation using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) ( F ) of F (2,411) = 4.82, p  < 0.01. Molnár et al. [ 3 ] found that the student grade affects domain-specific and complex problem-solving skills. They observed that the development of problem-solving skills was noticeable across grades in elementary school but stopped in secondary school. The fastest development of domain-specific problem-solving occurred in grades 7 and 8 [ 3 ], but the fastest development of complex problem-solving occurred in grades 5–7 [ 3 ]. No development was detected between grades 4 and 5 as well as grades 6 and 7 for domain-specific and complex problem-solving skills, respectively. Similarly, Greiff et al. [ 4 ] concluded that students developed problem-solving skills across grades 5–11 with older students being more skilled. However, the grade 9 students deviated from the development pattern, and their problem-solving skills dropped. The theories from Molnár et al. [ 3 ] and Greiff et al. [ 4 ] are the benchmark cases herein.

The above studies showed that problem-solving skills mostly developed during compulsory schooling and developed most quickly in specific grades. This indicates that specific development times can be targeted to enhance the problem-solving skills [ 3 ]. However, Jabor et al. [ 30 ] observed contradictory results showing statistically significant differences with small effects in mathematical performance between age groups: those under the age of 19 outperformed those over the age of 19 years old. Grissom [ 13 ] observed a negative correlation between age and school achievement that remained constant over time.

2.2. Effects of school location and gender on mathematical problem-solving skills

School location has been shown to affect mathematical achievement [ 9 , 14 ]. In 15 countries, students in rural schools performed considerably worse than students in urban schools in mathematics [ 9 , 10 ], science and reading [ 9 ]. In addition, Nepal [ 15 ] discovered that urban students significantly outperformed rural students in mathematical problem-solving skills ( t  = −5.11, p  < 0.001) and achievement ( t  = −4.45, p  < 0.001) using the results of an independent sample t -test (t). However, other countries have found that rural students outperformed urban students in mathematics [ 6 , 10 ]. These variations may be attributed to a lack of instructional resources (e.g., facilities, materials, and programs), professional training (e.g., poorly trained teachers), and progressive instruction [ 6 ]. The results of Williams's study [ 10 ] serve as the basis for the current study.

Gender differences in mathematics have received attention because studies show that male students outperform female students on higher-level cognitive tasks [ 31 ]. This is a shift from a meta-analysis study that found gender differences in mathematics to be insignificant and favored female students [ 32 ]. At the college level, female students slightly outperform male students in computation while male students outperform female students in problem solving. However, no gender differences have been observed among elementary and middle school students. This result was strengthened by other meta-analysis studies [ 7 , 8 ], which concluded that there was no gender difference in mathematical performance and problem-solving skills [ 15 , [33] , [35] , [34] ]. Gender similarity in mathematics is achieved when equal learning opportunities and educational choices are provided and the curriculum is expanded to include the needs and interests of the students [ 16 , 18 , 31 ].

From a sociological perspective, gender similarity in mathematics makes sense. If there is a gender difference in mathematics, this has been attributed to science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) being stereotyped as a male domain [ 8 ]. Stereotypes influence beliefs and self-efficacy of students and perceptions of their own abilities [ 8 , 19 ]. This is the reason for the low interest of female students in advanced mathematics courses [ 18 , 19 ]. However, Halpern et al. [ 16 ] found that more female students are entering many occupations that require a high level of mathematical knowledge. Moreover, Anjum [ 36 ] found that female students outperformed male students in mathematics. This may be because female students prepared better than the male students before the test and were more thorough [ 36 , 37 ]. The study of Anjum [ 36 ] is one of the basis cases of the current study.

Differences in brain structure support the argument that there are gender differences in mathematical performance [ 16 , 17 ]. Females have less brain lateralization (i.e., symmetric left and right hemispheres), which helps them perform better verbally. Meanwhile, males have more brain lateralization, which is important for spatial tasks [ 17 ]. In addition, the male hormone testosterone slows the development of the left hemisphere [ 16 ], which improves the performance of right brain-dominant mathematical reasoning and spatial tasks.

3.1. Instrumentation

In this study, a science-related mathematical problem-solving test was used. This is a mathematics essay test where the problems are in the form of scenarios related to environmental management. Problems are solved by using technology as a tool (e.g., calculator, grid paper). The test was developed in an interdisciplinary STEM framework, and it is targeted toward grades 7–9. There were six scenarios in total: some were given to multiple grades, and others were specific to a grade. They included ecofriendly packaging (grade 7), school park (grade 7), calorie vs. greenhouse gas emissions (grades 7–9), floodwater reservoir (grade 8), city park (grades 8–9), and infiltration well (grade 9). These scenarios cover topics such as number and measurement, ratio and proportion, geometry, and statistics. Every scenario had a challenge, and students were provided with eight metacognitive prompt items to help them explore their problem-solving skills.

The test was administered by using paper and pencils for a 3-h period with a break every hour. At the end of the test, students were asked to fill in their demographic information. Each prompt item had a maximum score of 5 points: a complete and correct answer (5 points), a complete answer with a minor error (4 points), an incomplete answer with a minor error (3 points), an incomplete answer with a major error (2 points), and a completely wrong and irrelevant answer (1 point). Each scenario had a maximum total score of 40 points.

The test was validated to determine whether it contained good and acceptable psychometric evidence. It had an acceptable content validity index (CVI >0.67), moderate intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) (rxx = 0.63), and acceptable Cronbach's alpha (α = 0.84). The construct validity indicated all scenarios and prompt items were fit (0.77 ≤ weighted mean square ≤1.59) with an acceptable discrimination value (0.48 ≤ discrimination value ≤ 0.93), acceptable behavior of the rating score, and good reliability (scenario reliability = 0.86; prompt item reliability = 0.94).

3.2. Participants

The test was administered to grades 7–9 students in east Java, Indonesia (n = 1067). The students were selected from A-accreditation schools in urban and rural areas; random classes were selected for each grade. The majority of the students were Javanese (95.01%), with the remainder being Madurese (3.3%) and other ethnicities. Table 1 describes the demographics of the participants.

Demographic characteristics of participants.

3.3. Data analysis

Data were collected between July and September 2022. Prior to data collection, ethical approval was sought from the institutional review board (IRB) of the Doctoral School of Education, University of Szeged and was granted with the ethical approval number of 7/2022. In addition, permission letters were sent to several schools to request permission and confirm their participation. The test answers of the students were scored by two raters – the first author of this study and a rater with master's degree in mathematics education – to ensure that the rating scale was consistently implemented. The results showed good consistency with an ICC of 0.992 and Cronbach's alpha of 0.996.

The scores from one of the raters were converted to a logit scale by weighted likelihood estimation (WLE) using the ConQuest software. A logit scale provides a consistent value or meaning in the form of intervals. The logit scale represents the unit interval between locations on the person–item map. WLE was chosen rather than maximum likelihood estimation (MLE) because WLE is more central than MLE, which helps to correct for bias [ 38 ]. The WLE scale was represented by using descriptive statistics to profile the students' mathematical problem-solving skills in terms of the percentage, mean score ( M ) and standard deviation ( SD ) for each phase. The WLE scale was also used to describe common difficulties for each phase. The development of students’ mathematical problem-solving skills across grades was presented by a pirate plot, which is used in R to visualize the relationship between 1 and 3 categorical independent variables and 1 continuous dependent variable. It was chosen because it displays raw data, descriptive statistics, and inferential statistics at the same time. The data analysis was performed using R studio version 4.1.3 software with the YaRrr package. A one-way ANOVA was performed to find significant differences across grades. An independent sample t -test was used to analyze significant differences based on gender and school location. The descriptive statistics, one-way ANOVA test, and independent sample t -test were performed using the IBM SPSS Statistics 25 software.

4.1. Student profiles

The scores of students were converted to the WLE scale, where a score of zero represented a student with average ability, a positive score indicated above-average ability, and a negative score indicated below-average ability. A higher score indicated higher ability. The mean score represented a student with average mathematical problem-solving skills ( M  = 0.001, SD  = 0.39). Overall, 52.1% of students had a score below zero. The distribution of scores among students was predominantly in the interval between −1 and 0. When the problem-solving process was analyzed by phase, the results showed that exploring and understanding were the most mastered problem-solving skills ( M  = 0.24, SD  = 0.51). Only 27.9% of students had below-average scores for the exploring and understanding phases, which indicates that they mostly understood the given problem and recognized the important information. However, the problem-solving skills decreased with higher phases. The students had below-average abilities in the phases of representing and formulating ( M  = −0.01, SD  = 0.36), planning and executing ( M  = −0.15, SD  = 0.41), and monitoring and reflecting ( M  = −0.16, SD  = 0.36). About 57.9% of the students had below-average scores for the representing and formulating phase, which indicates that they had problems making hypotheses regarding science phenomena, representing problems in mathematical form, and designing a prototype. The obvious reason for their difficulty with making hypotheses was that they did not understand simple concepts of science (e.g., CO 2 vs. O 2 ). In the planning and executing phase, 66.8% of the students failed to achieve a score greater than zero. This happened because they failed to apply mathematical concepts and procedures. Because they were unable to plan and execute a strategy, this affected the next phase of the problem-solving process. In the monitoring and reflecting phase, 68.0% of the students had a below-average score.

4.2. Development of mathematical problem-solving skills across grades

The development of the mathematical problem-solving skills of the students across grades was observed based on the increase in the mean score. The problem-solving skills developed from grade 7 to grade 8. The students of grade 7 had a mean score of −0.04 while grade 8 students had the highest mean score of 0.03. The students in grades 7 and 8 also showed more varied problem-solving skills than the grade 9 students did. In contrast, the grade 9 students showed a different pattern of development, and their mean score dropped to 0.01. Although the difference was not large, further analysis was needed to determine its significance.

Fig. 1 displays the development of the mathematical problem-solving skills of the students. The dots represent raw data or WLE scores. The middle line shows the mean score. The beans represent a smoothed density curve showing the full data distribution. The scores of the students in grades 7 and 9 were concentrated in the interval between −0.5 and 0. However, the scores of the grade 8 students were concentrated in the interval between 0 and 0.5. The scores of the students in grades 7 and 8 showed a wider distribution than those of the grade 9 students. The bands which overlap with the line representing the mean score, define the inference around the mean (i.e., 95% of the data are in this interval). The inference of the WLE score was close to the mean.

Fig. 1

Differences in students' mathematical problem-solving skills across grades.

Note : PS: Problem-Solving Skills of Students.

The one-way ANOVA results indicated a significant difference among the problem-solving skills of the students of grades 7–9 ( F (1,066) = 3.01, p  = 0.046). The students of grade 8 showed a significant difference in problem-solving skills and outperformed the other students. The students of grades 7 and 9 showed no significant difference in their mathematical problem-solving skills. Table 2 presents the one-way ANOVA results of the mathematical problem-solving skills across grades.

One-way ANOVA results of the mathematical problem-solving across grades.

Note. Post hoc test: Dunnett's T3. 7, 8, and 9: subsample grade. <: direction of significant difference ( p  < 0.05).

Fig. 2 shows the development of the mathematical problem-solving skills of the students across grades based on school location and gender. The problem-solving skills of the urban students increased from a mean score of 0.07 in grade 7 to 0.14 in grade 8. However, the mean score of urban students in grade 9 dropped. In contrast, the mean scores of the rural students increased continuously with grade. The improvements were significant for both the rural ( F (426) = 10.10, p  < 0.001) and urban ( F (639) = 6.10, p  < 0.01) students. For the rural students, grade 9 students showed a significant difference in problem-solving skills. In contrast, urban students in grades 8 and 9 showed significant differences in problem-solving skills but not in grade 7.

Fig. 2

Differences in students' mathematical problem-solving skills across grades and different demographic backgrounds.

(a) Differences in students grade 7 of mathematical problem-solving skills across grades and different demographic backgrounds

(b) Differences in students grade 8 of mathematical problem-solving skills across grades and different demographic backgrounds

(c) Differences in students grade 9 of mathematical problem-solving skills across grades and different demographic backgrounds

Note: WLE_PS: The students' problem-solving skills in WLE scale; F: Female; M: Male; ScLoc: School location; R: Rural; U: Urban.

When divided by gender, both female and male students showed improvements in their problem-solving skills from grades 7 and 8. However, female students in grade 9 showed a stable score while the scores of male students in grade 9 declined. Only male students in grade 7 showed a significant difference in the mean score. In urban schools, the scores of male and female students increased and decreased, respectively, from grade 7 to grade 8. Male students in rural schools showed an increase in score from grade 7 to grade 9. However, the scores of female students in rural schools decreased from grade 7 to grade 8. Table 3 presents the one-way ANOVA results for the mathematical problem-solving skills of the students considering gender and school location.

One-way ANOVA results for mathematical problem-solving skills across grades and different demographic backgrounds.

Fig. 2 shows that the distributions of the male and female scores of students were similar for every grade except rural grade 9 students. The scores of the rural female students were concentrated in the interval between 0 and 0.5 while the scores of the rural male students were mostly below 0. The scores of rural students in grade 7 and urban students in grade 9 (both male and female) were concentrated in the interval between −0.5 and 0. The scores of urban students in grades 7 and 8 were concentrated in the interval between −0.5 and 0.5.

Fig. 3 shows a detailed analysis of the development of mathematical problem-solving skills across grades for each phase of the problem-solving process. Similar patterns were observed in the exploring and understanding and the representing and formulating phases: the mean score increased from grade 7 to grade 8 but decreased from grade 8 to grade 9. Grade 8 students had the highest mean score and differed significantly from the scores of students in other grades.

Fig. 3

Differences in students' mathematical problem-solving skills in every phase across grades: (1) Exploring & understanding, (2) Representing & formulating, (3) Planning & executing, (4) Monitoring & reflecting.

(a) Differences in students' mathematical problem-solving skills in exploring and understanding phase

(b) Differences in students' mathematical problem-solving skills in representing and formulating phase

(c) Differences in students' mathematical problem-solving skills in planning and executing phase

(d) Differences in students' mathematical problem-solving skills in monitoring and reflecting phase

Note: WLE_Exp_Un: The WLE score in exploring and understanding; WLE_Rep_For: The WLE score in representing and formulating; WLE_Plan_Ex: The WLE score in planning and executing; WLE_Mon_Ref: The WLE score in monitoring and reflecting.

The scores of the students for the planning and executing phase increased with grade. However, the difference was only significant at grade 9. Grades 7 and 8 students showed an increase in score, but the improvement was not significant. There was no pattern detected in the monitoring and reflecting phase. The score was stable for grades 7 and 8 students but improved for grade 9 students. The mean score for each phase and the one-way ANOVA results are presented in Table 4 .

One-way ANOVA results for every phase of problem-solving across grades.

Fig. 3 shows that the distributions of the problem-solving skills of the students were similar across grades and phases. However, the distributions were different for grade 9 students in the representing and formulating, planning and executing, and monitoring and reflecting phases, where 95% of the data were in the interval between −0.5 and 0.5.

4.3. Effects of demographic background

4.3.1. school location.

The mathematical problem-solving skills of the students differed significantly based on school location. Urban students scored higher than rural students. The results of the t -test for mathematical problem-solving skills based on school location are presented in Table 5 .

T-test results for mathematical problem-solving skills based on school location.

The effects of the school's location on the performances of male and female students were analyzed. The results showed that the scores of the female students differed significantly based on school location ( t (613) = −6.09, p  < 0.001). Female students in urban schools ( M  = 0.18, SD  = 0.39) outperformed female students in rural schools ( M  = −0.08, SD  = 0.37). Similar results were observed for male students with urban students ( M  = −0.01, SD  = 0.35) outperforming rural students ( M  = −0.12, SD  = 0.39) by a significant margin ( t (382.764) = −3.25, p  < 0.01).

When analyzed by grade, grades 7 and 8 students contributed to the difference based on school location with t (377.952) = −6.34, p  < 0.001 and t (300.070) = −5.04, p  < 0.001, respectively. Urban students in grades 7 and 8 performed significantly better than their rural counterparts did. However, there was no significant difference between rural and urban students in grade 9 ( t (354) = 0.71, p  = 0.447).

4.3.2. Gender

Male and female students showed a significant difference in their mathematical problem-solving skills. Overall, female students outperformed male students. The detailed results of the independent sample t -test for mathematical problem-solving skills based on gender are presented in Table 6 .

T-test results for mathematical problem-solving skills based on gender.

The results were analyzed to determine whether the school location contributed to the gender difference. The gender difference was most significant among urban students ( t (596.796) = −4.36, p  < 0.001). Female students from urban schools ( M  = 0.12, SD  = 0.39) outperformed male students from urban schools ( M  = −0.01, SD  = 0.35). There was no significant difference between female and male students from rural schools ( t (425) = −1.31, p  = 0.191).

Grades 7 and 9 students contributed to the gender difference with t (372.996) = −3.90, p  < 0.001 and t (354) = −2.73, p  < 0.01, respectively. Female students in grades 7 and 9 outperformed their male counterparts. However, there was no significant gender difference among grade 8 students ( t (329) = −0.10, p  = 0.323).

5. Discussion

The mathematical problem-solving skills of the students were categorized as average. In addition, the difficulties of students increased in line with the problem-solving phase. Fewer students failed the exploring and understanding phase than the subsequent phases. This confirms the results of previous studies indicating that more students failed further along the problem-solving process [ 25 , 26 ]. Because the problem-solving process is sequential, students who have difficulty understanding a problem will fail the subsequent phases [ 27 ].

The development of mathematical problem-solving skills was evaluated according to the mean WLE score. The mathematical problem-solving skills of the students developed from grade 7 to grade 8 based on the increase in their mean scores. However, the development dropped in grade 9. This agrees with previous results that concluded that higher grades had the highest problem-solving skills, but the fastest skill development took place in grades 7–8 after which it dropped [ 3 , 4 ]. These results indicate that the mathematical problem-solving skills of the students should improve and be strengthened in grades 7–8, which will help them perform better in grade 9.

In this study, the effects of the demographic background of the students were analyzed in detail, which is an aspect missing from previous studies. The results showed that the mathematical problem-solving skills of urban students increased from grade 7 to grade 8 but decreased in grade 9. The same pattern was found among male and female students. However, a different pattern was observed for rural students, where the skills of grade 9 students continued to increase. The different patterns may be attributed to a structural reorganization of cognitive processes at a particular age [ 3 ]. However, more research is needed on the effects of the demographic backgrounds of students on mathematical problem-solving skills. These results were different from previous results because the previous studies only analyzed the development in general, without focusing on their demographic background. Hence, different patterns of development were observed when it was thoroughly examined.

Because solving problems is a cognitive process, the development of problem-solving skills for particular phases and processes needed to be analyzed. The students showed the same pattern for knowledge acquisition (i.e., exploring and understanding, and representing and formulating phases), with an increase in skill from grade 7 to grade 8 but a decrease in grade 9. However, the students showed increasing skill in knowledge application (i.e., planning and executing, as well as monitoring and reflecting phases) across grades. This means that the difference between the mean scores in grade 9 was not significant across phases. Grade 9 students had lower scores than grade 8 students for the knowledge acquisition phase but higher scores for the knowledge application phase. In contrast, the gap between the mean scores of grades 7 and 8 was large across phases.

These results proved that there is a significant difference in the mathematical problem-solving skills of students based on their demographic backgrounds. The urban students outperformed rural students, which confirms the results of previous studies [ 9 , 10 , 15 ]. The difference can be attributed to the availability of facilities, teacher quality, and interactive teaching and learning instruction [ 6 ]. In Indonesia, the policies for the public educational system for middle schools are set at the municipal level. This means that each city has its own policies for teacher training, teacher recruitment, teaching and learning processes, facilities, etc. Urban schools mostly have stricter policies as well as various programs to help students improve their knowledge and skills. In addition, they have supportive facilities for teaching and learning. This unequal environment is the strongest reason for the difference in mathematical problem-solving skills.

The results were analyzed in detail to observe which groups in the rural and urban schools contributed to the difference. Both male and female students in urban schools performed better than their counterparts in rural schools did. In addition, urban students in grades 7 and 8 outperformed their rural counterparts. There was no significant difference between urban and rural students in grade 9. This may be because grade 9 is the last grade in middle school, so students have to prepare for high school entrance requirements, including exam and/or grade point average scores. Hence, both rural and urban schools focus much effort on the teaching and learning process in this grade.

In this study, the female students surprisingly had better mathematical problem-solving skills than the male students did. This confirmed the results of the meta-analysis by Hyde et al. [ 32 ] and study by Anjum [ 36 ], which found that female students slightly outperformed male students in mathematics. This difference may be because of motivation and attitude [ 39 , 40 ]. Female Indonesian students are typically more diligent, thorough, responsible, persistent, and serious with their tasks.

A detailed analysis was performed to evaluate which group of students contributed to the gender differences. The results showed that female students outperformed male students in urban schools. This may be because male students at urban schools typically display an unserious attitude toward low-stake tests. In addition, female students outperformed their male counterparts in grades 7 and 9. The reason for this difference requires further analysis.

6. Conclusion

Studying the problem-solving skills of students is crucial to facilitating their development. In this study, the conclusions are presented as follows:

  • • The mathematical problem-solving skills of the students were categorized as average. More students failed at higher phases of the problem-solving process.
  • • Students showed development of their mathematical problem-solving skills from grade 7 to grade 8 but a decline in grade 9. The same pattern was detected across grades for urban students, both female and male. However, the problem-solving skills of rural students increased with the grade.
  • • A similar development was observed for the individual problem-solving phases. In the knowledge acquisition phase, the problem-solving skills of the students developed from grade 7 to grade 8 but decreased in grade 9. However, problem-solving skills increased across grades in the knowledge application phase.
  • • The school location was shown to have a significant effect on the mathematical problem-solving skills of the students. Urban students generally outperform students in rural schools. However, gender and grade contributed to differences in mathematical problem-solving skills based on school location. Female and male urban students in grades 7 and 8 outperformed their rural counterparts.
  • • In general, female students outperformed male students in mathematical problem-solving skills, particularly those from urban schools and in grades 7 and 9.

The sampling method and the number of demographic backgrounds limited the scope of this study. Only students from A-accreditation schools were selected because higher-order problem-solving skills were considered assets. Moreover, the study only included three demographic factors: grade, gender, and school location. More demographic information, such as school type, can be added (public or private schools). Hence, future studies will need to broaden the sample size and consider more demographic factors. Despite these limitations, this study can help teachers determine the best period for enhancing the development of mathematical problem-solving skills. Moreover, the differences in mathematical problem-solving skills due to demographic background can be used as a basis for educational policymakers and teachers to provide equal opportunity and equitable education to students.

Author contribution statement

Ijtihadi Kamilia Amalina: Conceived and designed the experiments; Performed the experiments; Analyzed and interpreted the data; Contributed reagents, materials, analysis tools or data; Wrote the paper.

Tibor Vidákovich: Conceived and designed the experiments; Contributed reagents, materials, analysis tools or data.

Funding statement

This work was supported by University of Szeged Open Access Fund with the grant number of 6020.

Data availability statement

Additional information.

No additional information is available for this paper.

Declaration of competing interest

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

  • Our Mission

Illustration of teacher and students drawing a nautilus

11 Real World Math Activities That Engage Students

Bridging the gap between abstract math concepts and real life experiences can make the subject accessible and relevant for kids.

During a unit on slope, José Vilson’s students just weren’t getting it, and their frustration was growing. The former middle school math teacher began brainstorming creative ways to illustrate the concept. “I kept thinking, ‘My students already understand how this works—they just don’t know that they know,’” Vilson writes in a recent article for Teacher2Teacher . “How can I activate knowledge they don’t believe they have?”

Then he thought about a hill a couple of blocks from school that his students “walk up every day to get to the subway.” He tacked up paper and began sketching stick figures on the hill. “One was at the top of the hill, one was halfway up, one was near the bottom skating on flat ground, and one was on a cliff,” writes Vilson, now the executive director of EduColor. “Which of these figures will go faster and why?” he asked his students. “That got my kids laughing because, of course, my stick figures weren’t going to hang in the MoMA.” Still, his sketch got them thinking and talking, and it provided a simple stepping stone that “gave that math relevance and belonging in their own lives,” Vilson concludes. 

“It’s not unusual for students to walk into our classrooms thinking that math belongs to people who are smarter, who are older, or who aren’t in their immediate circle,” Vilson writes. “But every time I teach math in a way that’s accessible and real for my students, I’m teaching them: ‘The math is yours.’”

To build on Vilson’s idea, we posted on our social channels asking teachers to share their favorite strategies for connecting math to students’ experiences and lives outside of school. We received hundreds of responses from math educators across grade levels. Here are 11 teacher-tested ideas that get students seeing and interacting with the math that surrounds them each day.

Hunt for clues

Coordinate systems can feel abstract to some students—but using coordinates to navigate a familiar space can solidify the concept in a relevant and fun way. “Before starting a unit on coordinates, I make gridded maps of the school—I make them look old using tea staining —and send my students off on a treasure hunt using the grid references to locate clues,” says Kolbe Burgoyne, an educator in Australia. “It’s meaningful, it’s fun, and definitely gets them engaged.”

Budget a trip

Students enjoy planning and budgeting for imaginary trips, teachers tell us, offering ample opportunities to practice adding, subtracting, and multiplying large numbers. In Miranda Henry’s resource classroom, for example, students are assigned a budget for a fictional spring break trip; then they find flights, hotels, food, and whatever else they’ll need, while staying within budget.

Math teacher Alicia Wimberley has her Texas students plan and budget a hypothetical trip to the Grand Canyon. “They love the real world context of it and start to see the relevance of the digits after the decimal—including how the .00 at the end of a price was relevant when adding.” One of Wimberley’s students, she writes, mixed up his decimals and nearly planned a $25,000 trip, but found his mistake and dialed back his expenses to under $3,000.

Tap into pizza love

Educators in our audience are big fans of “pizza math”—that is, any kind of math problem that involves pizza. “Pizza math was always a favorite when teaching area of a circle,” notes Shane Capps. If a store is selling a 10-inch pizza, for example, and we know that’s referring to its diameter, what is its total area? “Pizza math is a great tool for addition, subtraction, multiplication, word problems, fractions, and geometry,” another educator writes on our Instagram. There are endless pizza-based word problems online. Here’s a simple one to start, from Jump2Math : “The medium pizza had six slices. Mom and Dad each ate one slice. How much pizza is left?”

Break out the measuring cups

Lindsey Allan has her third-grade students break into pairs, find a recipe they like online, and use multiplication to calculate how much of each ingredient they’d need in order to feed the whole class. The class then votes on a favorite recipe, and they write up a shopping list—“which involves more math, because we have to decide, ‘OK, if we need this much butter for the doubled recipe, will we need three or four sticks, and then how much will be left over?’” Allan writes. “And then it turns out students were also doing division without even realizing!” 

Sometimes, a cooking mistake teaches students about proportions the hard way. “Nobody wants a sad chocolate chip cookie where you doubled the dough but not the chocolate chips,” adds teacher Holly Satter.

Heading outdoors is good for kids’ bodies , of course, but it can also be a rich mathematical experience. In second grade, kids can head out to measure perimeters, teacher Jenna McCann suggests—perhaps of the flower boxes in the school garden. If outdoors isn’t an option, there’s plenty of math to be found by walking around inside school—like measuring the perimeter of the tables in the cafeteria or the diameters of circles taped off on the gym floor.

In Maricris Lamigo’s eighth-grade geometry class, “I let [students] roam around the school and take photos of things where congruent triangles were applied,” says Lamigo. “I have students find distances in our indoor courtyard between two stickers that I place on the floor using the Pythagorean theorem,” adds Christopher Morrone, another eighth-grade teacher. In trigonometry, Cathee Cullison sends students outside “with tape measures and homemade clinometers to find heights, lengths, and areas using learned formulas for right and non-right triangles.” Students can make their own clinometers , devices that measure angles of elevation, using protractors and a few other household items.

Plan for adult life

To keep her math lessons both rigorous and engaging, Pamela Kranz runs a monthlong project-based learning activity where her middle school students choose an occupation and receive a salary based on government data. Then they have to budget their earnings to “pay rent, figure out transportation, buy groceries,” and navigate any number of unexpected financial dilemmas, such as medical expenses or car repairs. While learning about personal finance, they develop their mathematical understanding of fractions, decimals, and percents, Kranz writes.

Dig into sports stats

To help students learn how to draw conclusions from data and boost their comfort with decimals and percentages, fourth-grade teacher Kyle Pisselmyer has his students compare the win-loss ratio of the local sports team to that of Pisselmyer’s hometown team. While students can struggle to grasp the relevance of decimals—or to care about how 0.3 differs from 0.305—the details snap into place when they look at baseball players’ stats, educator Maggierose Bennion says.

March Madness is a great source of real world data for students to analyze in math class, says sixth-grade math teacher Jeff Norris. Last March, Norris decorated his classroom like a basketball court, then had his students do basic statistical analysis—like calculating mean, median, and mode—using March Madness data, including individual game scores and the total win rate of each team. “We also did some data collection through our own basketball stations to make it personally relevant,” Norris says; students lined up in teams to shoot paper balls into a basket in a set amount of time, recorded their scores in a worksheet, and then examined the scoring data of the entire class to answer questions about mean, median, mode, range, and outliers.

Go on a (pretend) shopping spree

“My students love any activities that include SHOPPING!” says Jessie, a sixth-grade teacher who creates shopping-related problems using fake (or sometimes real) store ads and receipts. Her students practice solving percentage problems, and the exercise includes opportunities to work with fractions and decimals.

To get students more engaged with the work, math educator Rachel Aleo-Cha zeroes in on objects she knows students are excited about. “I make questions that incorporate items like AirPods, Nike shoes, makeup, etc.,” Aleo-Cha says. She also has students calculate sales tax and prompts them to figure out “what a 50% off plus 20% off discount is—it’s not 70% off.”

Capture math on the fly

Math is everywhere, and whipping out a smartphone when opportunities arise can lead to excellent content for math class. At the foot of Mount Elbert in Colorado, for example, math teacher Ryan Walker recorded a short word problem for his fourth- and fifth-grade students. In the video, he revealed that it was 4:42 a.m., and it would probably take him 249 minutes to reach the summit. What time would he reach the summit, he asked his students—and, assuming it took two-thirds as long to descend, what time would he get back down?

Everyday examples can be especially relatable. At the gas station, “I record a video that tells the size of my gas tank, shows the current price of gas per gallon, and shows how empty my gas tank is,” says Walker. “Students then use a variety of skills (estimation, division, multiplying fractions, multiplying decimals, etc.) to make their estimate on how much money it will cost to fill my tank.”

Connect to social issues

It can be a powerful exercise to connect math to compelling social issues that students care about. In a unit on ratios and proportions, middle school teacher Jennifer Schmerler starts by having students design the “most unfair and unjust city”—where resources and public services like fire departments are distributed extremely unevenly. Using tables and graphs that reflect the distribution of the city’s population and the distribution of its resources, students then design a more equitable city.

Play entrepreneur

Each year, educator Karen Hanson has her fourth- and fifth-grade students brainstorm a list of potential business ideas and survey the school about which venture is most popular. Then the math begins: “We graph the survey results and explore all sorts of questions,” Hanson writes, like whether student preferences vary with age. Winning ideas in the past included selling T-shirts and wallets made of duct tape.

Next, students develop a resource list for the business, research prices, and tally everything up. They calculate a fair price point for the good they’re selling and the sales quantity needed to turn a profit. As a wrap-up, they generate financial statements examining how their profits stack up against the sales figures they had projected.

HELP OTHER TEACHERS OUT!

We’d love this article to be an evolving document of lesson ideas that make math relevant to kids. So, teachers, please tell us about your go-to activities that connect math to kids’ real world experiences.

Cubic directed graphs with application

  • Original Research
  • Published: 25 March 2024

Cite this article

  • Mohammed M. Ali Al-Shamiri 1 ,
  • Uzma Ahmad 2 ,
  • Afeefa Maryam 2 &
  • Muhammad Akram   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-7217-7962 2  

The purpose of this article is to present the Laplacian energy of cubic graphs and cubic directed graphs by analyzing the eigenvalues of the adjacency matrix and the Laplacian matrix. The proposed energy concept is discussed through mathematical and graphical models. Some useful properties related to cubic graph energies are studied, including their upper and lower bounds. The cubic fuzzy preference relationship is defined, and a multi-criteria decision-making model using the cubic fuzzy preference relationship is designed to select the best IT company for investment based on the energy of the cubic graph. Furthermore, an algorithm for solving the decision problem is described and the structure of the strategy is illustrated.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

mathematics in problem solving and decision making

Data availability

No data were used to support this study.

Ahmad, U., Batool, T.: Domination in rough fuzzy digraphs with application. Soft Comput. 27 (5), 2425–2442 (2023)

Article   Google Scholar  

Ahmad, U., Nawaz, I.: Directed rough fuzzy graph with application to trade networking. Comput. Appl. Math. 41 (8), 366 (2022)

Article   MathSciNet   Google Scholar  

Ahmad, U., Nawaz, I.: Wiener index of a directed rough fuzzy graph and application to human trafficking. J. Intell. Fuzzy Syst. 44 (1), 1479–1495 (2023)

Ahmad, U., Sabir, M.: Multicriteria decision-making based on the degree and distance-based indices of fuzzy graphs. Granul. Comput. 8 , 793–807 (2023)

Akram, M., Ahmad, U., Al-Shamiri, M.M.A., Shareef, A.: Algorithms for computing Pythagorean fuzzy average edge connectivity of Pythagorean fuzzy graphs. J. Appl. Math. Comput. 70 , 375–416 (2024)

Akram, M., Ilyas, F., Saeid, A.B.: Certain notions of Pythagorean fuzzy graphs. J. Intell. Fuzzy Syst. 36 (6), 5857–5874 (2019)

Akram, M., Habib, A., Ilyas, F., Dar, J.M.: Specific types of Pythagorean fuzzy graphs and application to decision-making. Math. Comput. Appl. 23 (3), 42 (2018)

MathSciNet   Google Scholar  

Akram, M., Naz, S., Davvaz, B.: Simplified interval-valued Pythagorean fuzzy graphs with application. Complex Intell. Syst. 5 (2), 229–253 (2019)

Akram, M., Davvaz, B.: Strong intuitionistic fuzzy graphs. Filomat 26 (1), 177–196 (2012)

Akram, M., Naz, S.: Energy of Pythagorean fuzzy graphs with applications. Mathematics 6 (8), 136 (2018)

Akram, M., Dudek, W.A.: Interval-valued fuzzy graphs. Comput. Math. Appl. 61 (2), 289–299 (2011)

Al-Hawary, T., Al-Shalaldeh, S., Akram, M.: Certain matrices and energies of fuzzy graphs. TWMS J. Appl. Eng. Math. 2 , 2146–2147 (2021)

Google Scholar  

Anjali, N., Mathew, S.: Energy of a fuzzy graph. Ann. Fuzzy Math. Inform. 6 (3), 455–465 (2013)

Atanassov, K.T.: Intuitionistic fuzzy sets. Fuzzy Sets Syst. 20 (1), 87–96 (1986)

Balakrishnan, R.: The energy of a graph. Linear Algebra Appl. 387 , 287–295 (2004)

Basha, S.S., Kartheek, E.: Laplacian energy of an intuitionistic fuzzy graph. Indian J. Sci. Technol. 8 (33), 1–7 (2015)

Chen, Q.J.: Matrix representations of fuzzy graphs. Math. Pract. Theory 1 , 41–46 (1990)

Ding, B.: Constrained robust model predictive control via parameter-dependent dynamic output feedback. Automatica 46 (9), 1517–1523 (2010)

Fahmi, A., Amin, F., Abdullah, S., Ali, A.: Cubic fuzzy Einstein aggregation operators and its application to decision-making. Int. J. Syst. Sci. 49 (11), 2385–2397 (2018)

Fang, G., Ahmad, U., Ikhlaq, S., Asgharsharghi, L.: Multi-attribute group decision making based on spherical fuzzy Zagreb energy. Symmetry 15 (8), 1536 (2023)

Fang, G., Ahmad, U., Rasheed, A., Khan, A., Shafi, J.: Planarity in cubic intuitionistic graphs and their application to control air traffic on a runway. Front. Phys. 11 , 1254647 (2023)

Guan, R., Yao, S., Liu, L., Zhu, X., Man, K.L., Yue, Y., Yue, Y.: Mask-VRDet: A robust riverway panoptic perception model based on dual graph fusion of vision and 4D mmWave radar. Robot. Auton. Syst. 171 , 104572 (2024)

Gutman, I.: The energy of a graph: old and new results. In: Algebraic Combinatorics and Applications, pp. 196–211. Springer (2001)

Gutman, I., Zhou, B.: Laplacian energy of a graph. Linear Algebra Appl. 414 (1), 29–37 (2006)

Hameed, S., Ahmad, U.: Minimal energy tree with 4 branched vertices. Open Chem. 17 , 198–205 (2019)

Indulal, G., Vijayakumar, A.: Energies of some non-regular graphs. J. Math. Chem. 42 , 377386 (2007)

Jun, Y.B., Kim, C.S., Yang, K.O.: Cubic Sets. Ann. Fuzzy Math. Inform. 4 (1), 83–98 (2011)

Kauffman, A.: Introduction to la Theorie des Sous-emsembles Flous. Masson et Cie 1 (1973)

Meenakshi, A. R.: Fuzzy Matrix Theory and Applications. MJP Publishers (2008)

Meng, S., Meng, F., Zhang, F., Li, Q., Zhang, Y., Zemouche, A.: Observer design method for nonlinear generalized systems with nonlinear algebraic constraints with applications. Automatica 162 , 111512 (2024)

Mordeson, J.N., Chang-Shyh, P.: Operations on fuzzy graphs. Inf. Sci. 79 (3–4), 159–170 (1994)

Muhiuddin, G., Takallo, M., Jun, Y.B., Borzooei, R.A.: Cubic graphs and their application to a traffic flow problem. Int. J. Comput. Intell. Syst. 13 (1), 1265–1280 (2020)

Muhiuddin, G., Hameed, S., Maryam, A. Ahmed, U.: Cubic Pythagorean fuzzy graphs. J. Math. 2022 , 1144666. https://doi.org/10.1155/2022/1144666

Nawaz, I., Ahmad, U.: Certain Concepts in directed rough fuzzy graphs and application to mergers of companies. Fuzzy Inf. Eng. 15 (3), 248–273 (2023)

Patra, N., Mondal, S., Pal, M., Mondal, S.: Energy of interval-valued fuzzy graphs and its application in ecological systems. J. Appl. Math. Comput. 68 , 3327–3345 (2022)

Praba, B., Chandrasekaran, V.M., Deepa, G.: Energy of an intuitionistic fuzzy graph. Italian J. Pure Appl. Math. 32 , 431–444 (2014)

Rashid, S., Yaqoob, N., Akram, M., Gulistan, M.: Cubic graphs with application. Int. J. Anal. Appl. 16 (5), 733–750 (2018)

Rashmanlou, H., Muhiuddin, G., Amanathulla, S.K., Mofidnakhaei, F., Pal, M.: A study on cubic graphs with novel application. J. Intell. Fuzzy Syst. 40 (1), 89–101 (2021)

Rahimi, S.S., Fayazi, F.: Laplacian energy of a fuzzy graph. Iran. J. Math. Chem. 5 (1), 1–10 (2014)

Rosenfeld, A.: Fuzzy graphs. In: Fuzzy Sets and Their Applications to Cognitive and Decision Processes, pp. 77–95. Academic Press (1975)

Shahzadi, S., Rasool, A., Santos-Garcia, G.: Methods to find strength of job competition among candidates under single-valued neutrosophic soft model. Math. Biosci. Eng. 20 (3), 4609–4642 (2023)

Shahzadi, S., Rasool, A., Sarwar, M., Akram, M.: A framework of decision making based on bipolar fuzzy competition hypergraphs. J. Intell. Fuzzy Syst. 41 (1), 1319–1339 (2021)

Wang, Y.M., Fan, Z.P.: Fuzzy preference relations: aggregation and weight determination. Comput. Ind. Eng. 53 (1), 163–172 (2007)

Yager, R. R.: Pythagorean fuzzy subsets. In: 2013 Joint IFSA World Congress and NAFIPS Annual Meeting (IFSA/NAFIPS), pp. 57–61. IEEE (2013)

Zadeh, L.A.: Fuzzy sets. Inf. Control 8 (3), 338–353 (1965)

Zadeh, L.A.: Similarity relations and fuzzy orderings. Inf. Sci. 3 (2), 177–200 (1971)

Zhou, B.: On the energy of a graph. Kragujevac J. Sci. 26 , 5–12 (2004)

Download references

Acknowledgements

The first author extends his appreciation to the Deanship of Scientific Research at King Khalid University for funding this work through Large Groups (Project under Grant No. RGP.2/32/44).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Mathematics, Faculty of Science and Arts, Mahayl Assir, King Khalid University, Abha, Saudi Arabia

Mohammed M. Ali Al-Shamiri

Department of Mathematics, University of the Punjab, New Campus, Lahore, Pakistan

Uzma Ahmad, Afeefa Maryam & Muhammad Akram

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Muhammad Akram .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Al-Shamiri, M.M.A., Ahmad, U., Maryam, A. et al. Cubic directed graphs with application. J. Appl. Math. Comput. (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12190-024-02046-y

Download citation

Received : 24 February 2024

Revised : 05 March 2024

Accepted : 06 March 2024

Published : 25 March 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12190-024-02046-y

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Cubic directed graph
  • Laplacian energy
  • Laplacian matrix
  • Cubic fuzzy preference
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. Difference Between Problem Solving and Decision Making

    mathematics in problem solving and decision making

  2. Problem-solving and Decision-making

    mathematics in problem solving and decision making

  3. Master Your Problem Solving and Decision Making Skills

    mathematics in problem solving and decision making

  4. Decision Making and Problem Solving Questions and Answers

    mathematics in problem solving and decision making

  5. Problem-Solving Process in 6 Steps

    mathematics in problem solving and decision making

  6. Use Effective Problem Solving and Decision-Making to increase

    mathematics in problem solving and decision making

VIDEO

  1. Olimpiad mathematics problem solving categories 3

  2. 1.1 Mathematics & Problem Solving (part 2)

  3. Critical Thinking Skills for Turbulent Times

  4. Keynote: The New Thing Generative AI Does

  5. Analytical Skills Uncovered!

  6. How to Use ABT Framework for More Persuasive Communication by Park Howell

COMMENTS

  1. Applying Mathematics to Solve Decision Problems

    Zabarankin's research focuses on the modeling of risk preferences (the tendency to seek or avoid risks) and optimization (selecting the best alternative) when dealing with decision problems (essentially, questions to be answered). "Decision problems are everywhere, in civilian and military applications ranging from investment science, where ...

  2. 2.10: Problem Solving and Decision Making

    Occasionally you will be asked for your opinion or experience for decisions others are making. To be effective in all of these circumstances, it is helpful to understand some principles about decision making. First, define who is responsible for solving the problem or making the decision.

  3. Mathematical Decision Making

    In mathematical decision theory, it is common practice to quantify actions by assigning a number g ( a) to the action a, and to define \ (a_1\unlhd a_2\) if and only if g ( a 1 ) ≤ g ( a 2 ), provided that such a function g exists. Indeed, the existence of the function g depends on what requirements we impose on the preference relation ...

  4. PDF Decision Making and Problem Solving

    improving our problem-solving and decision-making capabilities. In psychology, economics, mathematical statistics, operations research, political science, artificial intelligence, and cognitive science, major research gains have been made during the past half century in understanding problem solving and decision making.

  5. Problem Solving and Mathematical Modeling

    In order to develop the problem-solving skills, logical and analytical skills are the main pillars. Logical thinking enables thinking in terms of cause and consequences in sequential manner initiating the train of thoughts. Logical thinking also helps in decision making, problem solving as well as mathematical modeling of the problem.

  6. PDF Problem solving in mathematics

    Therefore, high-quality assessment of problem solving in public tests and assessments1 is essential in order to ensure the effective learning and teaching of problem solving throughout primary and secondary education. Although the focus here is on the assessment of problem solving in mathematics, many of the ideas will be directly transferable ...

  7. Mathematics and algorithms for intelligent decision-making

    To successfully apply mathematics and algorithms as part of real-world decision-making requires careful mathematical modelling and data collection. An inherent property of many decision problems of practical relevance is that they are computationally challenging. ... Solving such a problem within a reasonable amount of time often requires the ...

  8. Problem-Solving and Decision-Making

    Problem-solving is a more analytical process than decision-making. Problem-solving is more process-related, while decision-making is more contextual. Problem-solving is directed at a specific goal or discrete answer. Problem-solving and decision-making may have consequences that are not always predictable or sequential.

  9. Mathematical Methods for Decision Making and Optimization

    The goal of this Special Issue is to collect the most recent developments in mathematical decision-making and optimization techniques for a variety of engineering, science, and management applications. Both original research pieces and review papers are welcome to be submitted to this Special Issue. Neural networks.

  10. Algorithms for Decision Making

    Hardcover. $95.00. Hardcover. ISBN: 9780262047012. Pub date: August 16, 2022. Publisher: The MIT Press. 700 pp., 8 x 9 in, MIT Press Bookstore Penguin Random House Amazon Barnes and Noble Bookshop.org Indiebound Indigo Books a Million.

  11. When and why people perform mindless math

    Abstract. In this paper, we show that the presence of numbers in a problem tempts people to perform mathematical operations even when the correct answer requires no math, which we term "mindless math". In three pre-registered studies across two survey platforms (total N = 3,193), we investigate how mindless math relates to perceived problem ...

  12. PDF The Mathematics Educator A Problem With Problem Solving: Teaching

    Problem solving theory and practice suggest that thinking is more important to solving problems than ... decision making, conceptualizing, and information processing (Ellis, 2005). Although scholars and practitioners often ... method is mostly associated with problem solving in mathematics. The last is a more contemporary version developed by ...

  13. Does mathematics training lead to better logical thinking and ...

    Mathematics is often promoted as endowing those who study it with transferable skills such as an ability to think logically and critically or to have improved investigative skills, resourcefulness and creativity in problem solving. However, there is scant evidence to back up such claims. This project tested participants with increasing levels of mathematics training on 11 well-studied rational ...

  14. The Adolescent Brain: Learning, Reasoning, and Decision Making

    The Adolescent Brain raises questions relevant to young people's educational and health outcomes, as well as to neuroscience research. This book examines the ways in which brain development impacts reasoning and decision-making in adolescents, from the use and function of memory and representation, to judgment, mathematical problem-solving and ...

  15. How to Improve Problem-Solving Skills: Mathematics and Critical

    Decision Making: Choose the most suitable method to address the problem. Implementation: Put the chosen solution into action. Evaluation: Reflect on the solution's effectiveness and learn from the outcome. By embedding these steps into mathematical education, we provide students with a structured framework. When they wonder about how to ...

  16. Problem Solving in Mathematics Education

    In mathematics education, the mathematicians' work and developments in disciplines as psychology became relevant to relate problem-solving activities and the students' learning of mathematics. Schoenfeld ( 1985) suggests that open critiques (Kline 1973) to the new math and the back-to-basic reforms in the USA were important to focus on ...

  17. Mathematics probability and decision-making

    decision-making and how mathematical-logical assessments of probability can im-prove problem-solving abilities by provid-ing a greater basis for making decisions. In memoriam Mr. William Betz, formerly a specialist in mathematics for the City School District of Rochester, New York, died during the summer of 1965. He was a charter member of the Na-

  18. (PDF) Problem Solving and Decision Making

    November 2016. Sergei Talanker. In the following paper I argue that problem-solving and decision-making are just different aspects of the same multi-stage goal-oriented cognitive process. I prove ...

  19. Roicki: Math Can Help Teach Students to Make Good Decisions ...

    Decision-making and mathematical problem-solving. Math really is the perfect content area for building good decision-making skills. Solving math problems involves more than simply finding a single correct answer. Students make decisions throughout the process. ... When solving problems, they also need to attend to mathematical relationships in ...

  20. Problem Solving in Mathematics Education

    1.1 Role of Heuristics for Problem Solving—Regina Bruder. The origin of the word heuristic dates back to the time of Archimedes and is said to have come out of one of the famous stories told about this great mathematician and inventor. The King of Syracuse asked Archimedes to check whether his new wreath was really made of pure gold. Archimedes struggled with this task and it was not until ...

  21. Development and differences in mathematical problem-solving skills: A

    2.1. Mathematical problem-solving skills and their development. Solving mathematical problems is a complex cognitive ability that requires students to understand the problem as well as apply mathematical concepts to them [].Researchers have described the phases of solving a mathematical problem as understanding the problem, devising a plan, conducting out the plan, and looking back [, , , , ].

  22. 11 Real World Math Activities That Engage Students

    March Madness is a great source of real world data for students to analyze in math class, says sixth-grade math teacher Jeff Norris. Last March, Norris decorated his classroom like a basketball court, then had his students do basic statistical analysis—like calculating mean, median, and mode—using March Madness data, including individual ...

  23. Cognition: Mental Representations, Problem Solving, and Decision Making

    Problem solving uses mental models, forms a basis for learning, and can be supported in a variety of ways. Decision making is a more punctuated form of problem solving, made about and with systems. It is not always as clear or accurate as one would like (or expect), and there are ways to support and improve it.

  24. Math anxiety relates positively to metacognitive insight into

    The current study reports a pre-registered investigation into the interrelations between mathematics anxiety, metacognition and mathematical decision-making. Although this question has already received some attention in previous work, reliance on self-report measures of metacognition has hindered its interpretation. Here, a novel experimental mathematical decision-making task was used in which ...

  25. Cubic directed graphs with application

    The purpose of this article is to present the Laplacian energy of cubic graphs and cubic directed graphs by analyzing the eigenvalues of the adjacency matrix and the Laplacian matrix. The proposed energy concept is discussed through mathematical and graphical models. Some useful properties related to cubic graph energies are studied, including their upper and lower bounds. The cubic fuzzy ...