• Select country and document
  • Answer questions
  • Download and print

German Assignment Agreement

This document is accurate and up to date! It was last reviewed by a lawyer in April 2024 .

This agreement is used when one party (the assignor) agrees to assign a contract, or the rights in a contract, or another income/object to a second party (the assignee). In exchange for the assignment, the assignee may give the assignor money or personal property, or forgive a debt or obligation.

Simply answer the questions below to personalize your Assignment Agreement

This legal document is also known as:

This form also known as: contract assignment agreement, transfer contract rights form, assignment of cntractural rights

More Testimonials

Other Popular Documents

  • Promissory Note
  • Bill of Sale
  • Confidentiality / NDA
  • Residential Lease
  • Power of Attorney
  • Employment Agreement
  • Child Travel Consent
  • Child Medical Consent
  • Child-Care Contract
  • Employment Contract
  • Sales Contract
  • Service Contract

Related External Links

Governing jurisdiction:, assignor details:, assignor information:, first assignor's information:, second assignor's information:, assignee details:, assignee information:, first assignee's information:, second assignee's information:, nature of assignment:, contract is with:, consideration:, contract is with:, warranties:, signatures required:, date of signing:, free german assignment agreement templates.

The sample documents below are provided for informational purposes only.

  • They are NOT up to date;
  • They have NOT been reviewed by a Lawyer;
  • They may NOT be legally valid or enforceable.

We strongly recommend obtaining an attorney reviewed Assignment Agreement directly from us .

Free samples are coming soon!

Assignment Agreement

Jump to section.

An assignment agreement is a contract that authorizes a person to transfer their rights, obligations, or interests in a contract or property to another person. It serves as a means for the assignor to delegate duties and advantages to a third party while the assignee assumes those privileges and obligations. This blog post will discuss assignment agreement, its purpose, essential elements, and implementation practices.

Key Functions of an Assignment Agreement

Below are some key functions of an assignment agreement.

  • Facilitating Clear Transfer of Rights and Obligations: Assignment agreement plays a vital role in diverse industries and business transactions by facilitating a transparent transfer of rights and obligations between parties. These agreements encompass intellectual property rights, contractual duties, asset ownership, and other legal entitlements. By clearly defining the assignment's scope and nature, both parties can ensure a smooth transition without any uncertainties.
  • Ensuring Protection of Interest: Another important objective of the assignment agreement is safeguarding the assignor and assignee's interests. These agreements provide a legal framework that protects the assignee's rights while relieving the assignor of responsibilities and liabilities associated with the assigned asset or contract. This protection ensures that neither party faces unexpected consequences or disputes during or after the assignment.
  • Outlining Consensus on Terms and Conditions : Assignments often involve intricate terms and conditions, necessitating mutual understanding between the assignor and assignee. Assignment agreement serves as binding documents that outline the assignment's terms and conditions, including payment terms, timelines, performance expectations, and specific requirements. By reaching a consensus on these details, both parties can minimize potential conflicts and align their expectations.
  • Complying with Legal Laws: Ensuring legal compliance and enforceability is an important objective of the assignment agreement. Also, it is prudent to create these documents according to the relevant rules, regulations, and industry requirements. By adhering to legal guidelines, the assignment agreement becomes a robust legal instrument that provides a solid foundation for potential legal action in case of breaches or disputes.
  • Maintaining Confidentiality and Non-Disclosure: Many assignments involve confidential information, proprietary knowledge, or trade secrets that require protection. An objective of the assignment agreement is to establish clear guidelines regarding the confidentiality and non-disclosure of such information. These guidelines define the scope of confidential information, specify restrictions on its use or disclosure, and outline the consequences of any breaches. By ensuring clarity in these aspects, the agreement protects the interests of both parties and fosters a sense of trust .

Best Practices for Crafting an Assignment Agreement

Assignment agreements are vital in different business transactions, transferring rights and obligations from one person to another. Whether it's a merger, acquisition, or contract assignment, implementing an assignment agreement needs thorough consideration and adherence to best practices to ensure a seamless and lawfully sound process. Below are some key practices to follow when implementing an assignment agreement.

  • Identifying the Parties Involved: The initial step in implementing an assignment agreement is to identify the parties participating in the assignment agreement. It is vital to accurately define the assignor, who will transfer the rights, and the assignee, who will receive them. The assignment agreement should include precise details of both parties' names and contact information.
  • Defining the Scope and Extent of Assignment: It is imperative to define the assignment's scope and extent clearly to prevent potential disputes or ambiguity in the future. It specifies the rights, benefits, and obligations transferred from the assignor to the assignee. In addition, specific details such as intellectual property rights, contractual obligations, and any relevant limitations or conditions should be explicitly outlined.
  • Reviewing and Understanding Existing Contracts or Agreements: Assignment agreements often transfer rights and obligations from preexisting contracts or agreements. It is essential to thoroughly review and comprehend these existing contracts to facilitate a seamless transfer. Identifying any provisions restricting or prohibiting assignment is important and should be addressed accordingly. Seeking legal advice is advisable to ensure compliance with contractual obligations.
  • Obtaining Consent from Relevant Parties: In some cases, obtaining consent from third parties directly affected by the transfer of rights and obligations may be necessary. Also, it is important to identify these parties and obtain their consent in writing if required. Failure to get permission may lead to legal complications and a potential breach of contract .
  • Crafting a Comprehensive Assignment Agreement: Upon collecting all relevant data, it is time to create a comprehensive assignment agreement. This agreement should utilize unambiguous language to define the rights and obligations transferred, specify the effective date of the assignment, and outline any other relevant terms and conditions. Engaging legal professionals specializing in contract law is highly recommended to ensure the agreement's legal validity and enforceability.
  • Seeking Legal Advice and Performing Review: It is important to seek legal advice and conduct a thorough review before finalizing the assignment agreement. Experienced attorneys can provide valuable insights, identify potential risks, and ensure compliance with applicable laws and regulations. The legal review helps minimize the likelihood of errors or oversights that could result in future disputes or legal challenges.
  • Executing and Recording the Assignment Agreement: Once the assignment agreement has been reviewed and approved, both parties should implement the document by signing it. Also, to enhance its enforceability, it is advisable to have the assignment agreement witnessed or notarized, depending on the jurisdiction's legal requirements. Additionally, maintaining a record of the executed contract is essential for future reference and as evidence of the assignment.
  • Communicating the Assignment: Effective communication of the assignment to all relevant parties is important after executing the assignment agreement. Stakeholders, such as employees, clients, suppliers, and contractors, should be notified about the transfer of rights and obligations. It ensures a smooth transition and minimizes potential disruptions or misunderstandings.
  • Documenting and Ensuring Compliance: Lastly, it is imperative to maintain proper documentation and ensure ongoing compliance with the assignment agreement's terms. Keeping copies of all relevant documents, including the assignment agreement, consent, and communications related to the assignment, is important. Regularly reviewing and monitoring compliance with the assignment agreement allows for prompt resolution of any issues and helps maintain a transparent and accountable process.

assignment agreement in german

Key Terms for Assignment Agreements

  • Assignor: The individual or entity that transfers their rights, responsibilities, or interests to another party using an assignment agreement. And by doing so, the assignor relinquishes any claims and duties associated with the assigned property, contract, or legal entitlements.
  • Assignee: The individual or entity that receives the rights, interests, or obligations through an assignment agreement. The assignee assumes the transferred rights and responsibilities, essentially taking on the role of the assignor.
  • Obligor: Refers to the party bound by a duty or obligation under a contractual or legal agreement. In an assignment agreement, the obligor is the party whose performance or obligations are assigned to the assignee.
  • Assignable Rights: These are the specific rights or interests that can be transferred from the assignor to the assignee via an assignment agreement. These include intellectual property rights, contractual rights, real estate interests, royalties, and other lawful entitlements.
  • Consideration: The value or benefit exchanged between the parties in an assignment agreement. Also, consideration is commonly paid in monetary payment, goods, services, or promises. It represents what each party gains or sacrifices as part of the assignment.
  • Notice of Assignment: A formal written notification provided by the assignor to the obligor, serving as a communication of the assignment of rights, interests, or obligations to the assignee. This notice establishes the assignee's rights and enables the obligor to fulfill their duties to the correct party.

Final Thoughts on Assignment Agreements

In a nutshell, assignment agreement plays an important role in business transactions, allowing for transferring of rights, duties, and interests between parties. Moreover, by understanding these objectives and addressing them through well-drafted assignment agreement, businesses and individuals can engage in assignments with confidence and clarity. Also, since an assignment agreement includes several legal complexities, it is rational to consult a professional attorney who can guide you through the process.

If you want free pricing proposals from vetted lawyers that are 60% less than typical law firms, Click here to get started. By comparing multiple proposals for free, you can save the time and stress of finding a quality lawyer for your business needs.

Meet some of our Assignment Agreement Lawyers

Garrett M. on ContractsCounsel

I am a solo-practitioner with a practice mostly consisting of serving as a fractional general counsel to start ups and growth stage companies. With a practical business background, I aim to bring real-world, economically driven solutions to my client's legal problems and pride myself on efficient yet effective work.

Billy Joe M. on ContractsCounsel

Billy Joe M.

I graduated from the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign in 2006 with a degree in Political Science, Finance, and Economics. I stayed around Champaign for law school and graduated in 2009. I then worked at a big law firm in downtown Chicago. It was boring, so I quit in early 2011. I thought that I could not be happy practicing law - I was wrong. After I quit the traditional law firm life, I began representing my own clients. I realize now that I love helping normal people, small business owners, and non-profits address a variety of legal issues. I hope to hear from you.

Phocus L. on ContractsCounsel

G'day, my name is Michele! I work with startups, entrepreneurs and small/medium-sized businesses across the country in a wide array of industries. I help them with all of their ongoing, daily legal needs. This includes entity formation, M&A, contract drafting and review, employment, asset sale & acquisition, and business sales or shareholder exits. I'm half-Australian, half-Italian, and I've lived the last 20+ years of my life in America. I've lived all over the USA, completing high school in the deep south, graduating cum laude from Washington University in St. Louis, and then cum laude from Georgetown University Law Center. After law school I worked for the Los Angeles office of Latham & Watkins, LLP. After four intense and rewarding years there, I left to become General Counsel and VP of an incredible, industry-changing start-up called Urban Mining Company (UMC) that manufactures rare earth permanent magnets. I now work for Phocus Law where I help run our practice focused on entrepreneurs, startups, and SMEs. I love what I do, and I'd love to be of help! My focus is on providing stress-free, enjoyable, and high-quality legal service to all of my clients. Being a good lawyer isn't enough: the client experience should also be great. But work isn't everything, and I love my free time. I've been an avid traveler since my parents put me on a plane to Italy at 9-months old. I'm also a music nut, and am still looking for that perfect client that will engage me to explain why Dark Side Of The Moon is the greatest album of all time. Having grown up in a remote, and gorgeous corner of Australia, I feel a strong connection to nature, and love being in the elements.

Nicholas A. on ContractsCounsel

Nicholas A.

I help small business owners build and protect their dreams. I always thought that I would just be a litigator. Then I joined an intellectual property clinic in law school. We were helping nonprofits and small businesses reach their goals. I fell in love with the work and decided to open my own firm so I could keep helping them. When I decided to start Victrix Legal, I decided that it would be a modern law firm designed to serve professionals. It would be different from every other law firm. In my experience, my law firms are designed to promote inefficiency and reactionary lawyering. Because in most firms, you make more money when you spend more time on a project. And you lose money if your client doesn't get sued. In my opinion, that's a built-in conflict of interest. My firm is different. I use flat fees for most basic projects to keep costs predictable for you and incentivize efficiency. I offer long-term advisory plans and legal audits to prevent issues from happening. I want my clients to see me as their business partner, not just the guy they call when they are in trouble. If any of that interests you, please reach out to me. I offer free consultations. Let's set aside some time and talk about what your legal needs are.

Gerald W. on ContractsCounsel

My clients know me as more than just an attorney. First and foremost, my background is much broader than that. Prior to attending the Valparaiso University School of Law, I earned a Master of Business Administration and ran a small business as a certified public accountant. Thanks to this experience, I possess unique insight which in turn allows me to better assist my clients with a wide range of business and tax matters today. In total, I have over 20 years of experience in financial management, tax law, and business consulting, and I’m proud to say that I’m utilizing the knowledge I’ve gained to assist the community of Round Rock in a variety of ways. In my current practice, I provide counsel to small to medium-sized businesses, nonprofit organizations, and everyday individuals. Though my primary areas of practice are estate planning, elder law, business consulting, and tax planning, I pride myself on assisting my clients in a comprehensive manner. Whenever I take on a new client, I make an effort to get to know them on a personal level. This, of course, begins with listening. It is important that I fully understand their vision so I can help them successfully translate it into a concrete plan of action that meets their goals and expectations. I appreciate the individual attributes of each client and know firsthand that thoughtful, creative, and customized planning can maximize both financial security and personal happiness. During my time as a certified public accountant, I cultivated an invaluable skill set. After all, while my legal education has given me a deep understanding of tax law, I would not be the tax attorney I am today without my background in accounting. Due to my far-reaching experience, I am competent in unraveling even the most complex tax mysteries and disputes. My CPA training benefits my estate planning practice, too. In the process of drafting comprehensive wills and trusts, I carefully account for every asset and plan for any tax burdens that may arise, often facilitating a much smoother inheritance for the heirs of my clients. Prior to becoming certified as a CPA, I made sure to establish a solid foundation in business both in and out of the classroom, and the acumen I’ve attained has served me well. Not only am I better able to run my own practice than I otherwise would be; I am able to help other small business owners fulfill their dreams, as well.

James David W. on ContractsCounsel

James David W.

I graduated from Harvard Law School and worked first for a federal judge and then a leading DC firm before starting a firm with a law school classmate. My practice focuses on company formations, early-stage investments, and mergers & acquisitions.

Anna K. on ContractsCounsel

Anna is an experienced attorney, with over twenty years of experience. With no geographical boundaries confining her practice, Anna works on corporate, healthcare and real estate transactions. Anna brings extensive big firm experience, garnered as an associate in the Miami office of the world's largest law firm, Baker and McKenzie, and the Miami office of the international law firm Kilpatrick Townsend. Her areas of expertise include: mergers and acquisitions, initial public offerings, private placements, healthcare transactions, corporate finance, commercial real estate transaction and acting as a general corporate counsel. Anna is certified to practice law in Florida and was admitted to the Florida Bar in 1998. Anna is also a Certified Public Accountant. She passed May 1995 CPA Exam on the first sitting. She is fluent in Russian (native).

Find the best lawyer for your project

How it works.

Post Your Project

Get Free Bids to Compare

Hire Your Lawyer

Business lawyers by top cities

  • Austin Business Lawyers
  • Boston Business Lawyers
  • Chicago Business Lawyers
  • Dallas Business Lawyers
  • Denver Business Lawyers
  • Houston Business Lawyers
  • Los Angeles Business Lawyers
  • New York Business Lawyers
  • Phoenix Business Lawyers
  • San Diego Business Lawyers
  • Tampa Business Lawyers

Assignment Agreement lawyers by city

  • Austin Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Boston Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Chicago Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Dallas Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Denver Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Houston Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Los Angeles Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • New York Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Phoenix Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • San Diego Assignment Agreement Lawyers
  • Tampa Assignment Agreement Lawyers

related contracts

  • 93a Demand Letter
  • Accounting Services Agreement
  • Accounts Receivable Purchase Agreement
  • Ad Agency Contract
  • Adhesion Contract
  • Advertising Services Agreement
  • Agency Agreement
  • Agency Contract

other helpful articles

  • How much does it cost to draft a contract?
  • Do Contract Lawyers Use Templates?
  • How do Contract Lawyers charge?
  • Business Contract Lawyers: How Can They Help?
  • What to look for when hiring a lawyer

assignment agreement in german

Quick, user friendly and one of the better ways I've come across to get ahold of lawyers willing to take new clients.

Contracts Counsel was incredibly helpful and easy to use. I submitted a project for a lawyer's help within a day I had received over 6 proposals from qualified lawyers. I submitted a bid that works best for my business and we went forward with the project.

I never knew how difficult it was to obtain representation or a lawyer, and ContractsCounsel was EXACTLY the type of service I was hoping for when I was in a pinch. Working with their service was efficient, effective and made me feel in control. Thank you so much and should I ever need attorney services down the road, I'll certainly be a repeat customer.

I got 5 bids within 24h of posting my project. I choose the person who provided the most detailed and relevant intro letter, highlighting their experience relevant to my project. I am very satisfied with the outcome and quality of the two agreements that were produced, they actually far exceed my expectations.

Want to speak to someone?

Get in touch below and we will schedule a time to connect!

Find lawyers and attorneys by city

Go to the homepage

German translation of 'assignment'

IPA Pronunciation Guide

Video: pronunciation of assignment

Youtube video

Examples of 'assignment' in a sentence assignment

Trends of assignment.

View usage for: All Years Last 10 years Last 50 years Last 100 years Last 300 years

Browse alphabetically assignment

  • assignation
  • assimilable
  • All ENGLISH words that begin with 'A'

Quick word challenge

Quiz Review

Score: 0 / 5

Image

Wordle Helper

Tile

Scrabble Tools

Image

To support our work, we invite you to accept cookies or to subscribe.

You have chosen not to accept cookies when visiting our site.

The content available on our site is the result of the daily efforts of our editors. They all work towards a single goal: to provide you with rich, high-quality content. All this is possible thanks to the income generated by advertising and subscriptions.

By giving your consent or subscribing, you are supporting the work of our editorial team and ensuring the long-term future of our site.

If you already have purchased a subscription, please log in

What is the translation of "assignment" in German?

"assignment" in german, assignment {noun}.

  • volume_up Abtretung
  • Aufgabengebiet
  • Beauftragung
  • Arbeitseinsatz
  • Festsetzung
  • zugewiesene Aufgabe
  • Übereignung
  • Übereignungsurkunde

assign {vb}

  • volume_up abtreten
  • beauftragen

assignability {noun}

  • volume_up Abtretbarkeit

assigned {adj.}

  • volume_up abgestellt

assignable {adj.}

  • volume_up abtretbar
  • übertragbar

Translations

  • "of property"
  • open_in_new Link to source
  • warning Request revision

assign [ assigned|assigned ] {verb}

Assigned {adjective}, assignable {adjective}.

  • "allottable"

Context sentences

English german contextual examples of "assignment" in german.

These sentences come from external sources and may not be accurate. bab.la is not responsible for their content.

Monolingual examples

English how to use "assignment" in a sentence, english how to use "assign" in a sentence, english how to use "assignability" in a sentence, english how to use "assigned" in a sentence, english how to use "assignable" in a sentence, english how to use "abtretbarkeit" in a sentence, collocations, "give the assignment" in german.

  • volume_up Geben Sie die Zuweisung
  • volume_up geben Sie den Auftrag

"accept the assignment" in German

  • volume_up Akzeptieren der Zuweisung
  • volume_up Annahme der Zuordnung

"assignment editors" in German

  • volume_up Zuordnungs-Editoren
  • volume_up Aufgaben-Editoren

Synonyms (English) for "assignment":

  • appointment
  • designation
  • duty assignment
  • transferable
  • transferrable
  • assigned transfer
  • assigned value
  • assigned work
  • assignee in bankruptcy
  • assignees in bankruptcy
  • assigning of resources
  • assigning transfer
  • assignment and assumption agreement
  • assignment back
  • assignment editors
  • assignment for security
  • assignment for the benefit of creditors
  • assignment form
  • assignment in advance
  • assignment in blank
  • assignment include
  • assignment involve

Moreover, bab.la provides the Japanese-English dictionary for more translations.

Social Login

assignment in other languages:

Cambridge Dictionary

  • Cambridge Dictionary +Plus

Translation of assignment – English–German dictionary

(Translation of assignment from the PASSWORD English–German Dictionary © 2014 K Dictionaries Ltd)

Examples of assignment

Translations of assignment.

Get a quick, free translation!

{{randomImageQuizHook.quizId}}

Word of the Day

Your browser doesn't support HTML5 audio

acting or speaking together, or at the same time

Alike and analogous (Talking about similarities, Part 1)

Alike and analogous (Talking about similarities, Part 1)

assignment agreement in german

Learn more with +Plus

  • Recent and Recommended {{#preferredDictionaries}} {{name}} {{/preferredDictionaries}}
  • Definitions Clear explanations of natural written and spoken English English Learner’s Dictionary Essential British English Essential American English
  • Grammar and thesaurus Usage explanations of natural written and spoken English Grammar Thesaurus
  • Pronunciation British and American pronunciations with audio English Pronunciation
  • English–Chinese (Simplified) Chinese (Simplified)–English
  • English–Chinese (Traditional) Chinese (Traditional)–English
  • English–Dutch Dutch–English
  • English–French French–English
  • English–German German–English
  • English–Indonesian Indonesian–English
  • English–Italian Italian–English
  • English–Japanese Japanese–English
  • English–Norwegian Norwegian–English
  • English–Polish Polish–English
  • English–Portuguese Portuguese–English
  • English–Spanish Spanish–English
  • English–Swedish Swedish–English
  • Dictionary +Plus Word Lists
  • PASSWORD English–German    Noun
  • Translations
  • All translations

Add assignment to one of your lists below, or create a new one.

{{message}}

Something went wrong.

There was a problem sending your report.

  • Special Collections
  • Submissions
  • Author Guidelines
  • Ensuring a Blind Review
  • Start Submission
  • Glossa Special Collections
  • Journal Policies
  • Publisher Policies
  • Editorial Team
  • Become a Reviewer

Structural and phonological cues for gender assignment in monolingual and bilingual children acquiring German. Experiments with real and nonce words

We investigate the acquisition of grammatical gender marking in German by monolingual children as well as German-Russian bilingual children who grow up in Germany as heritage speakers of Russian. We ask to what extent monolingual and bilingual children use phonological and/or structural cues to assign nominal gender, and to what extent they rely on lexical knowledge. To this end, we designed three experiments. Experiment 1 tests gender assignment with real nouns; Experiment 2 tests gender assignment to nonce nouns with the same set of noun-internal phonological cues as in Experiment 1, and in Experiment 3 we compare gender assignment to nonce nouns based on phonological vs. structural (agreement) cues when both types of cues are provided. Results show that children are significantly less successful when assigning gender to nonce nouns as compared to real nouns, which highlights the importance of lexical learning. At the same, we observe sensitivity to noun-internal phonological cues for both mono- and bilingual children. Bilingual children show similar patterns as monolingual children but different default strategies. For the bilingual children, we discuss the possibility of cue transfer from Russian to German. Finally, we observe that the role of structural (agreement) cues increases with age, while the role of noun-internal phonological cues decreases with age for the bilingual children, in line with previous findings from other languages.

gender, assignment, cues, German, Russian, cross-linguistic influence, bilingualism

How to Cite

Kupisch, T., Geiss, M., Mitrofanova, N. & Westergaard, M., (2022) “Structural and phonological cues for gender assignment in monolingual and bilingual children acquiring German. Experiments with real and nonce words”, Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 7(1). doi: https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5696

Download XML Download PDF

1. Introduction

One central debate in the field of early bilingual language acquisition concerns the nature and the extent of cross-linguistic influence (CLI), i.e., whether a bilingual child’s two languages develop autonomously or influence each other in a significant way. A recent meta-analysis by van Dijk et al. ( 2021 ) measured the effect of CLI in 750 simultaneous and early sequential bilingual children (from 26 different studies and 17 language combinations) and found “a significant small to moderate average effect size of CLI, indicating that CLI is part and parcel of bilingual development” (p. 1). However, further research is needed to establish in what way CLI is modulated by the linguistic distance and structural similarity between the languages involved. Moreover, it remains unclear if the effect of CLI is equally strong across different linguistic domains and features. In the current study we focus on the acquisition of grammatical gender in German by German-Russian bilingual children, considering the global effect of overall transparency of the gender systems in the two languages as well as the role of more local and fine-grained structural and phonological gender cues.

The number of studies on the acquisition of gender in bilingual children has increased massively over the past few years, and the results have often been interpreted as CLI, which may affect either gender discovery, i.e., the moment when children start marking gender on gender targets (e.g., articles or adjectives) or the rate of correct gender marking. The idea behind these studies is typically that a language with a more transparent gender marking system can influence a language with a less transparent system positively (resulting in acceleration) or, conversely, that a language with a less transparent gender marking system can affect a language with a more transparent system negatively (resulting in delays) (see Kupisch et al. 2002 ; Cornips & Hulk 2008 ; Hulk & van der Linden 2010 ; Eichler et al. 2013 ). For example, a language without a gender marking system, such as English, may have a delaying effect on the acquisition of gender in another language (e.g., Unsworth et al. 2014 ; Kaltsa et al. 2019 ). Conversely, a language with a transparent gender-marking system, such as Greek, may have an accelerating effect on the acquisition of gender in another language with a less-transparent gender-system ( Egger et al. 2018 ; Kaltsa et al. 2019 ).

In most of these studies, it is implicitly or explicitly assumed that languages differ in terms of how transparent their gender marking systems are. For example, Dutch and Norwegian have relatively non-transparent systems, which take long to be acquired (up to age 7, Rodina & Westergaard 2015 ; Egger et al. 2018 ), while Spanish, Italian and Russian have transparent systems, which are acquired fast with only a few errors after age 3–4 ( Pérez-Pereira 1991 ; Ceitlin 2009 ). The parameters referred to are typically (i) the transparency of morphological, phonological or semantic cues on the noun itself, (ii) the transparency of agreement marking on elements which are in a syntactic relation with the noun, (iii) how often gender is relevant (i.e., how many targets show gender marking), and (iv) the number of genders. As for the latter, it has been proposed that two-way systems with masculine and feminine can generally be acquired faster than systems with an additional neuter category ( Eichler et al. 2013 ).

While most of these studies make plausible assumptions about transparency based on the grammatical gender systems of the investigated target languages, children’s actual knowledge of the rules and regularities underlying these systems is rarely ever tested explicitly. Importantly, we would need to know what bilingual children’s knowledge of the gender system consists of in order to make assumptions about the properties that can potentially be transferred from one language system into the other. If, for example, children have merely lexical knowledge, e.g., knowing that the Italian noun sole is masculine, then transferring this knowledge would not be facilitative because translation equivalents often have different genders. 1 If, on the other hand, children have internalized knowledge that noun endings can be used to predict nominal gender, they might apply this more abstract knowledge to the other language. Therefore, in addition to investigating whether children assign gender successfully to existing nouns, we need to investigate whether they are sensitive to more general cues (phonological regularities of nouns belonging to the same gender) and exactly which cues they are sensitive to.

If we assume that languages differ gradually with respect to the predictability of noun-internal cues for gender assignment, we may set up a scale of transparency, where languages such as Italian, Spanish and Russian would be at one end of the scale (with a few highly predictive rules that may account for the majority of nouns in the language), while languages like Dutch and Norwegian would be at the other end of the scale (see Figure 1 ).

Figure 1

Predictability of gender marking noun-internal cues in selected languages.

Since children ultimately acquire all these gender systems, it should be possible to learn grammatical gender both by paying attention to cues and by rote-learning gender features of individual lexical items, with the former strategy being more relevant for Italian and Russian and the latter strategy for Dutch and Norwegian. Nevertheless, it has been shown in a number of studies that transparent systems are learned early, while less transparent ones lag behind (e.g., Kupisch et al. 2002 ; Rodina & Westergaard 2015 ; 2017 ; Egger at al. 2018 ). But what is the preferred strategy for children acquiring a language that is somewhere in between, such as German? Will the children be learning item by item for an extended period of time, or will they discover the statistical patterns for gender assignment from early on?

The paper is structured as follows. In section 2, we introduce gender assignment and agreement patterns in German, motivating the assumption that the gender marking system in German is semi-transparent, and we also provide an overview of previous research relevant to the paper. In section 3 we formulate the research questions and predictions. Sections 4–6 present one Experiment each. We discuss the results and conclude in sections 7 and 8.

2. Background

2.1 gender assignment and agreement in german.

German has a three-way gender system with masculine, feminine and neuter. Gender is marked on determiners (definite and indefinite articles, demonstratives, possessives), attributive adjectives and relative pronouns. Masculine is the most frequent and neuter the least frequent gender, although the exact numbers vary depending on the type of corpus (e.g., spoken vs. written, child-directed vs. adult-directed, basic vocabulary vs. advanced vocabulary). Schiller & Caramazza ( 2003 ) report 43% masculine, 38% feminine and 19% neuter gender, based on lemmas in the CELEX database (4164 monomorphemic nouns). For child speech, Binanzer ( 2017 ) reports 42% masculine, 31% feminine and 27% neuter; for basic vocabulary Wegener ( 1995 ) finds 44% masculine, 33% feminine and 23% neuter nouns. In many accounts on German, masculine is considered to be the default for gender assignment in German. The reason is not only its comparatively higher frequency –masculine is only slightly more frequent than feminine– but rather the assigned gender when assignment rules are in competition or when no specific gender assignment rules apply (e.g., Steinmetz 2006 ). On the other hand, Opitz & Pechmann ( 2016 ) have provided evidence that masculine gender requires increased processing load, which would speak against its default status. According to Köpcke & Zubin ( 2009, pp. 148–150 ), it is in fact possible to construct an argument for all three genders as the default in German, depending on theoretical position. Based on examples of headless NPs such as das   Schöne ‘the beautiful’ and reference to unknown objects ( Was ist denn   das ? ‘What’s that?‘), they claim that Neuter gender could be considered the ‘system default’, though admitting that such examples could also be genderfree. Corbett & Fraser ( 2000: 69f ) argue that such examples could be construed as showing the syntactic gender default in German. The syntactic default is generally assumed to be Neuter, based on presentative or expletive constructions ( das   ist meine Mutter ‘that is my mother’, es scheint … ‘it seems …’). Thus, there is no general consensus about the notion of default gender in German (see also Enger ( 2009 ) for a critical discussion of defaults, and Corteen ( 2019 ) for a discussion of defaults within Optimality Theory).

The most frequent elements on which gender marking is visible are articles, which are obligatory with singular count nouns. Gender interacts with number, definiteness and case, and there is considerable syncretism, which makes structural cues to nominal gender somewhat opaque. For example, gender is unambiguously marked on definite singular DPs in the nominative case, although the adjective is underspecified with respect to gender. In indefinite DPs in the nominative case, there is syncretism between neuter and masculine articles, but the adjective is unambiguously marked for gender (see Table 1 ). In the plural, there is no gender marking.

Examples of gender marking in German (nominative case).

Besides structural cues to nominal gender (i.e., elements in syntactic agreement with the noun), there are morphological, phonological and semantic cues on the noun itself ( Köpcke 1982 ; Köpcke & Zubin 1983 ). German generally follows the natural gender rule, in that nouns referring to male beings are masculine, and nouns referring to female beings are feminine. 2 Suffixes can be associated with particular genders, e.g., – chen and – lein are associated with neuter gender, even if there is a clash with natural gender, as illustrated by the examples Mädchen N ‘girl’ and Männlein N ‘little man’. There are numerous phonological cues to gender assignment, but many of them reflect probabilistic tendencies rather than rules. Amongst the most reliable ones for each gender are the following: Disyllabic nouns ending in [ə] are associated with feminine gender 90% of the time (e.g., Katze f ‘cat’), monosyllabic nouns starting with the onset cluster /ʃ/+C_ are associated with masculine gender 81% of the time (e.g., Strand M ‘beach’), and nouns ending in [ɛt] are associated with neuter 95% of the time (e.g., Bett N ‘bed’) (see, e.g., Köpcke 1982 ).

2.2 The acquisition of gender by German monolingual children

Gender is most frequently marked on articles, which is why first gender marking is closely related to first article use. Mills ( 1986: 63 ) reports first uses of definite articles in spontaneous child data around age 1;10–2;2, and Stern & Stern ( 1928: 44, 86 ) around 1;2–1;10. Articles are typically not gender marked at this point but reduced or underspecified forms, e.g., de . Such forms are often considered to be proto-articles whose function is to mark a syntactic position, while the acquisition of morphology lags behind (e.g., Bottari et al. 1993/1994 ). Sporadic use of gender-marked articles is reported from age 2;0–2;4 ( Mills 1986 ), 1;9 ( Bittner 2006 ), and 1;5 ( Szagun 2007 ), though with substantial individual variation. acquisition tends to be defined as the point when children cease to make gender-marking mistakes (90% accuracy). This happens around age 3;0 ( Mills 1986 ; Szagun et al. 2007 ).

Based on a single case study, Bittner ( 2006 ) argued that children’s early articles do not have a gender-marking function at all, but are used to mark syntactic function instead, with der referring to subjects and den to objects. Der in nominative position constitutes more than 90% of the forms used before age 2;4 regardless of the noun’s gender; die is overused between 2;5 and 2;11, and das is hardly ever overused. According to Bittner the gender system only emerges at 2;11, but the dissociation between masculine and neuter only by the age of 4. However, since her data are primarily presented in terms of case and only non-target uses are quantified, the study leaves open what the rate of correct gender marking was. Further, the early acquisition of case in German is not generally confirmed (e.g., Tracy 1986 ).

Based on longitudinal data, Mills ( 1986: 64 ) observed that the feminine article eine was the most common early form, also with nouns of masculine or neuter gender. When indefinite articles became frequent, ein and eine fluctuated, sometimes in combination with the same noun. Mills speculated that the incorrect use of the indefinite article may be due to “the high frequency of the ending – e in the article and adjective paradigms […] leading to an overgeneralization of the – e ending on all prenominal units.” Definites appeared later than indefinites. While Mills observed an association of monosyllabic nouns with der (p. 83) and of the schwa-ending with die (p. 85), errors suggested that not all assignment was cue based. Errors involved overuse of die , even with nouns containing masculine cues, e.g., Bagger (- er is a cue for masculine) and Hund (monosyllabic nouns tend to be masculine). Thus, overuse of die does not result from early acquisition of the feminine cue alone. Article omission was still frequent between the ages of two and three years. Since the children produced far more definite articles with feminine nouns than with nouns of the other two genders, Mills concluded that children are more confident with the feminine gender. Her account is largely in line with Stern & Stern ( 1928 ), who reported a distinction between masculine and feminine forms by 2;6 as well as gender errors resulting from overuse of die . Mills ( 1986 ) also investigated older children (ages 3;2–6;3) in an experimental situation, and results showed few incorrect productions of articles (5%), with none of the 6-year-olds producing any errors. There were comparatively fewer errors with feminine, and when errors were made with masculine and feminine, children rarely resorted to neuter. Feminine and neuter were rarely confused with one another.

Overall, the literature review indicates considerable individual variation, but some overall tendencies: (i) despite the challenges posed by the system, children’s development of gender in German appears to be comparatively fast (mostly before age 3), (ii) feminine articles emerge early and neuter articles late, (iii) there is some sensitivity to phonological noun-internal cues, and (iv) article omission or production of underspecified articles may be considered children’s strategies to avoid morphological markings of gender and case.

2.3 The acquisition of gender by German bilingual children

Studies on the acquisition of German by early bilinguals can be distinguished in terms of whether German is acquired simultaneously as an L1 or sequentially as an early L2. Another relevant distinction is whether German is acquired along with a language that has gender and, if so, how transparent gender marking in that language is.

In Müller’s ( 1999 ) study of three German-French simultaneous bilingual children, gender was marked from around 1;10–2;4. Initially, the children mostly omitted determiners, except for French un and German ein , which seemed to be used as numerals and irrespectively of the gender of the noun they preceded. Subsequently, they started using determiners productively and assigned gender based on phonological and semantic criteria. Gender assignment with definite articles was unproblematic, except for neuter, which occurred only from around age 2;4–2;8. Assignment and overgeneralization errors were determined by phonological and semantic properties. The children used feminine definite articles with nouns ending in schwa, even if these nouns were masculine, i.e., overusing the schwa-regularity. For instance, the children produced die F   Hase M ‘rabbit’ or die F   Affe M ‘monkey’; alternatively they used the correct gender but adjusted the noun ending to make assignment more rule-conform, as in der *Aff ‘the monkey’ (monosyllabic nouns being associated with masculine). One of the children also overused der with monosyllabic nouns ( der M   Haus N ‘house’, der M   Geld N ‘money’, der M   Bett N ‘bed’), indicating sensitivity to the tendency for monosyllabic nouns to be masculine. Neuter remained a problem until the age of 5, and neuter nouns were initially classified as masculine ( Müller 1999: 393 ).

Eichler, Jansen & Müller ( 2013 ) investigated gender marking in naturalistic data from 17 simultaneous bilingual children acquiring a Romance language (French, Italian or Spanish) along with German or another Romance language. They compared gender errors across all languages. The children were followed longitudinally between 2 and 3;6 years and gender marking was investigated based on article production. The bilingual children were more successful in their Romance languages than in German (and children acquiring Italian and Spanish had advantages over children acquiring French), although most of the children grew up in Germany. Knowledge of cues was not explicitly discussed, but the children were aware of the schwa-rule, as evident from overgeneralizations such as die F   Junge M ‘the boy’ (p. 565). Neuter was acquired late.

Also based on naturalistic data, Wegener ( 1995 ) studied the acquisition of gender by L1-Turkish, Polish, and Russian children acquiring German as an early L2. The children started out using bare nominals; their first articles had the function of marking indefinite (nonspecific) as opposed to definite reference. Ein was the most frequent indefinite article and die the most frequent definite one, arguably due to the high token frequency of these forms in the input. Subsequently, the children used articles for case-marking or argument-marking functions: r -forms for subjects, s -forms for direct objects and e -forms for plurals, somewhat similar to what is claimed by Bittner ( 2006 ) for monolingual German. The children eventually used articles successfully, according to Wegener primarily due to lexical learning (p. 15), but they also showed sensitivity to formal rules. For example, after only 13–15 months of exposure the L1-Polish and L1-Russian children assigned masculine gender to monosyllabic nouns and nouns in – er and feminine to nouns in schwa (pp. 16–17). The L1-Turkish children in grade 4 performed at chance, possibly due to the absence of gender in Turkish.

Dieser ( 2009 ) presented longitudinal naturalistic data (ages 1–6;6) and cross-sectional semi-naturalistic and experimental data (ages 3–10) from German-Russian children. She investigated both languages and compared simultaneous and sequential bilinguals. All learners, independently of type of bilingualism, passed through similar acquisition sequences in both languages, starting with a two-way gender system, feminine and masculine for Russian, feminine and non-feminine for German (see also Hopp 2011 ). Gender differentiation started with indefinite articles, before being extended to definite articles. Dieser concluded that monolinguals and bilinguals make use of lexical and rule-based learning in both languages, with a higher amount of rule-based learning in Russian. The German rules included the schwa-rule.

Ruberg ( 2013 ) compared German monolingual children (ages 3–5) and sequential bilingual children with different heritage languages (Polish, Turkish, Russian; ages 4;0–6;3). Based on real and nonce words, his results show that by the age of 4;0 monolingual children use correct definite and indefinite articles. The sequential bilingual children did not show target-like gender marking after 30 months of contact with German. When making assignment errors, both the monolingual and the sequential bilingual children defaulted to masculine, in line with Steinmetz’s ( 2006 ) claim that masculine is the default gender in German. Ruberg’s results further showed that both monolingual and bilingual children made use of morphophonological cues, assigning gender more often correctly if the nouns ended in schwa (associated with feminine) or were monosyllabic (associated with masculine). When the noun did not exhibit a prominent gender cue, children mainly used the default strategy. As in Wegener’s study, the L1 Turkish children did not attend to cues to the same extent as the two other bilingual groups.

Finally, Lemmerth & Hopp ( 2018 ) investigated lexical and syntactic CLI in simultaneous and sequential bilingual German-Russian children (mean age 8), focusing on German. Elicited production data showed that the children assigned gender correctly independently of whether the nouns have congruent or different genders in the children’s two languages. By means of eye-tracking, the authors further investigated whether the children used articles and adjectives to anticipate an upcoming noun. The simultaneous bilingual children behaved like monolingual controls, while the sequential bilinguals made use of gender marking only when the gender of the noun was equivalent in the two languages. Lemmerth & Hopp ( 2018 ) speculated that sequential bilinguals assign gender in the L2 by first accessing the lexicon of their L1, while there is no such influence in simultaneous bilinguals. This latter claim raises the interesting question on what basis the children drew a parallel between feminine, masculine and neuter gender in German and Russian. Unless the children had been taught German and Russian grammar explicitly, we believe that there is no reason for them to match specific genders one-to-one in the two languages.

In summary, although there is substantial variation in the acquisition of German gender by bilinguals, there are many indications that bilingual children show sensitivity to the same cues as monolingual children. The variation seems to be determined by the language combination, specifically the presence/absence and transparency of gender marking-rules in the other language, but it remains unclear what exactly can be transferred across languages. Table 2 summarizes some relevant studies on German.

Some studies on the acquisition of German (2L1=simultaneous bilinguals; eL2=early sequential bilinguals).

2.4 Grammatical gender in Russian and its acquisition

As mentioned in the Introduction, gender in Russian is mostly transparent ( Corbett 1982 ; 1991 ), with three rules that predict gender assignment to the majority of nouns in the language: In the nominative singular, masculine nouns typically end in a consonant (e.g., dom M ‘house’), most feminine nouns end in - a (e.g., lisá F ‘fox’), and most neuter nouns end in - o (e.g., vedró N ‘bucket’). There are also some phonological gender cues that are ambiguous, most notably affecting nouns that end in a palatalized consonant, which may be either masculine or feminine (e.g., gus’ M ‘goose’ or kost’ F ‘bone’), as well as nouns ending in schwa, which may be either feminine or neuter (e.g., parta F [partə] ‘desk’ or sito N [sitə] ‘sieve’). The majority of nouns ending in a schwa are feminine, leading to early overgeneralizations of schwa-final neuters to feminine ( Ceitlin 2009 ; Mitrofanova et al. 2018 ). Russian has no articles, but gender is marked as agreement on adjectives, demonstratives, quantifiers, some numerals and verbs in the past tense. According to Corbett ( 1991 ), masculine and feminine genders are almost equally frequent (46% and 41% respectively), with masculine being considered the default for gender assignment, while neuter is by far less frequent (13%).

Table 3 summarizes (morpho-)phonological gender cues in Russian, comparing them to those we introduced above for German. We remind the reader that there are more possible noun endings in both German and Russian, which are often predictive of gender, but we restrict ourselves to those relevant for the present study.

Comparison of (prominent) phonological rules in German and Russian.

The current study is a continuation of similar studies on gender assignment in bilingual populations acquiring two gender languages that differ with respect to transparency. Rodina & Westergaard ( 2017 ) elicited real words to investigate the gender system of Norwegian-Russian bilinguals and found that transparency plays a major role, as gender was acquired faster in Russian than in Norwegian, even though Russian was the children’s heritage language. There was no difference between the bilinguals and monolingual controls in the majority language (Norwegian), but the children’s performance in Russian was dependent on the amount of input. Unlike Polinsky ( 2008 ), who found that some (adult) heritage speakers of Russian in the US were developing a reduced gender system with only masculine and feminine, Rodina & Westergaard found that the children generally had a three-gender system and that only the children with the least amount of input defaulted to masculine, thus seemingly developing a Russian variety without gender. Mitrofanova, Rodina, Urek & Westergaard’s ( 2018 ) follow-up study used nonce words and showed that the heritage children were generally sensitive to the transparent phonological cues in Russian, and defaulting to masculine decraesed with a higher Cumulative Length of Exposure to Russian.

The German-Russian children in the current study have also been investigated in their heritage language Russian and compared with monolingual controls ( Mitrofanova et al. 2021 ), with the same three experiments as in the present study: real words, nonce words, and nonce words with mismatched cues (i.e., noun endings and adjectival morphology cueing for different genders). The findings show sensitivity to both transparent and ambiguous cues in Russian for both monolingual and bilingual children, but bilingual children used the masculine default to a considerably larger extent. For the ambiguous cues, all children displayed a preference for feminine with nouns ending in a schwa and sensitivity to more fine-grained cues for the nouns ending in palatals, such as the type of palatal consonant. In the mismatch experiment, the importance of the syntactic (agreement) cue increased significantly with age, while that of the noun-internal phonological cue decreased, echoing findings from French by Karmiloff-Smith ( 1979 ).

3 Research questions and predictions

Our literature review shows early sensitivity to gender based on naturalistic corpora, but there are several hints that not all regularities have been acquired by age 3, and that the acquisition of assignment regularities is still ongoing during the primary school years. This is plausible given the magnitude of regularities in German, their relatively low extension and the assumption that children postulate rules and regularities only when they have collected sufficient evidence in the input. Since bilingual children tend to develop their vocabularies more slowly than monolinguals (see Montanari et al., 2018 and Klassert et al., 2014 , for German-Russian children), they may need longer to generalize over a class of nouns and postulate a rule. Alternatively, given their smaller vocabularies, they may be more inclined to rely on formal cues from earlier on, especially if they have another language in which formal cues are more prominent. This motivates our first research question and corresponding prediction.

RQ1 : In a language with only semi-transparent gender cues/statistical tendencies, such as German, are children sensitive to phonological gender cues? When exposed to new nouns, do they default to one gender, and if so, which one? Prediction : Since monolingual German children have been found to produce few non-target-consistent gender forms in spontaneous speech after age 3, we predict that both mono- and bilingual children will show some sensitivity to certain phonological gender cues, despite the relatively low reliability of such cues in German. We also expect this sensitivity to be dependent on the relative reliability of the cue in the language and thus that the cue - e for F nouns would be the one where the children show the highest cue sensitivity. At the same time, given the generally low reliability of phonological cues in German, we also predict that the children to a large extent will use a default gender with nonce nouns. According to previous research both within gender typology and acquisition, M gender is the default in Russian and at least some children default to masculine also in German. We thus expect both mono- and bilingual children to some extent to default to M gender (cf. section 2.1 and 2.2).

As outlined in the introduction, many studies have argued for positive or negative CLI from one language to another leading to acceleration or delay. Here, we ask whether such cross-linguistic effects, if we find them, can be linked to transfer of experience with phonological cues:

RQ2 : Are there quantitative and/or qualitative differences between monolinguals and bilinguals, and, if so, can they be related to CLI from Russian? Prediction : Given the high reliability of (morpho-)phonological cues in Russian, we expect there to be a facilitative effect of Russian in the results of the bilinguals. That is, we predict that the bilinguals show a generally higher sensitivity to phonological cues in German compared to monolingual children. Thus, while the monolinguals might be more accurate than the bilinguals with existing words, we expect the bilinguals to score relatively higher with nonce words.

Finally, since we know from previous research that older children are more sensitive to structural cues, we speculate that the same might hold for German-learning children. However, they may show an even stronger tendency to rely on structural cues, since the phonological cues in German are weak. This motivates our third question.

RQ3 : Which strategy do German-speaking mono- and bilinguals use for gender assignment when exposed to conflicting cues – do they pay more attention to the phonological shape of the noun or structural cues on other targets, and does this change with age? Prediction : Given findings from other languages (L1 French, 2L1 Russian, see above), we predict that young children are more sensitive to the phonological cue on the noun early on, even when it is less reliable than structural cues. In accordance with previous research, we further predict that children will pay more attention to structural cues with increasing age.

4. Experiment 1 (real words)

The purpose of Experiment 1 was to establish whether bilingual children assign the target gender to nouns that reflect gender assignment regularities and whether they differ from monolinguals. Since we used specific cues for gender assignment in nonce words (Experiment 2), we wanted to establish whether the children knew the gender of real nouns that contained precisely these cues.

4.1 Participants

53 bilingual children (ages 3–11, mean 6,2) were recruited from a Russian language school in Singen, Hohentwiel (South Germany), a Russian daycare centre in Stuttgart, and four in Berlin. All children were heritage speakers of Russian growing up in Germany. These children spoke no other languages than German and Russian. 18 children came from German-Russian families (ages 3–11; mean 6.22; sd = 2.42), i.e., families in which one parent spoke Russian and the other German (henceforth ‘mixed’ families) and 35 from families in which both parents spoke Russian (ages 4–10, mean 6,22; sd = 1.92). Family type coincides with type of bilingualism, i.e., children from mixed families were simultaneous bilinguals and children from only Russian-speaking families were sequential bilinguals. Age of onset to German varied (range 0–6 years, mean 2.1). For most children first exposure to German was at the age of 3 years when entering kindergarten. 3 Additionally, we tested 24 monolingual German children (ages 3–9; mean 4.92; sd = 1.43).

Words in Experiment 1 (see Appendix 2) were based on selected gender assignment tendencies, as specified in Köpcke ( 1982 ), Köpcke & Zubin ( 1983 ), and Wegener ( 1995 ), cf. section 2.1 Disyllabic nouns ending in schwa were used for feminine, mono- and disyllabic nouns in /ɛt/ were used for neuter, and monosyllabic nouns starting with /ʃ/+C___ and ending in ___Nasal+C targeted masculine gender. The selected nouns were inanimate and concrete. There were 18 nouns in total, six per gender category. Since we targeted specific phonological cues and since we needed inanimate nouns that could be depicted, certain drawbacks could not be avoided. First, it was impossible to balance congruent and incongruent nouns within the three gender categories. All masculine nouns that fulfilled our selection criteria happened to have congruent translation equivalents in Russian, and most neuter nouns happened to be incongruent. For the feminine category in schwa, which is generally more frequent, congruent and incongruent nouns were balanced. Overall, 50% of the nouns were congruent. Second, the frequency of the noun in each gender category differed. Nouns in the feminine category tend to be the most frequent in child corpora, while most nouns in the neuter and masculine categories were of low frequency (see Table 1 in Appendix 2).

The experiment was framed in a story of Martians coming for their first visit to Earth. In Experiment 1, the Martians took pictures of objects they saw during their visit, but since they had no idea what these objects were called, the children had to tell the Martians the names of the objects. The children were seated in front of a PowerPoint presentation on a screen and their responses were audiotaped with an Olympus LS-11. The children saw two objects of different sizes and colours, and they were asked to name them. Then one object disappeared and the children had to guess which one, again using an article, an adjective and a noun. An example of the procedure is given in (1). Each trial targeted the elicitation of two indefinite DPs and one definite DP (relevant DPs in bold). The experimenters were native speakers of German and were carefully instructed on how to carry out the experiment. Preceding the experiment, there was a training session with three items, one of each gender.

  • Example procedure for Experiment 1
  • Pictures of a blue and red candle appear simultaneously on the screen
  • Experimenter:
  •        Was siehst du?
  •        (What do you see?)
  • Expected answer:
  • Eine F   blaue F   (Kerze) und eine F   rote F   Kerze .
  • (a blue (candle) and a red candle)
  • The red candle disappears
  •        Was ist verschwunden?
  •        (what disappeared?)
  • Die F   rote Kerze .
  • (the red candle)

4.3 Analysis

For a first overview, we classified all utterances in terms of whether the elicited gender target (definite or indefinite article or adjective) was realized or not. The gender targets were indefinite articles and adjectives (indefinite condition) or a definite article (definite condition). If the elicited target was provided by the child, the realization was coded as M, F or N. If the child did not provide the elicited gender target, we distinguished replacements of indefinite by definite articles and vice versa, article omissions and non-classifiable responses . The responses were not classifiable if the noun was changed or if no gender target was provided. An overview of the responses for the monolingual and bilingual groups is provided in Table 4 . Note that masculine and neuter responses cannot be distinguished in the indefinite condition due to syncretism.

Real word experiment, overview of responses (predominant answer pattern in bold).

For further analysis, gender assignment was coded based on the definite article, but some adjustments were necessary because the children did not always produce the elicited target.

If no article was produced, the adjective in the indefinite condition was used as an indicator of assigned gender, but only if it was clearly marked as masculine (e.g., roter ) or as neuter (e.g., rotes ). Potentially feminine marked adjectives (e.g., rote ) were not considered, because the form can represent all genders in definite DPs (cf. Table 1 ). Further, Table 2 suggests that the bilingual children still have problems with definiteness markings. 4 Note that this procedure potentially biased the analysis against feminine marked DPs.

If neither an article (or other determiner) nor an adjective marked as neuter or masculine was produced, we refrained from coding any assigned gender.

Responses in which the children used a different noun than the intended one were excluded, unless the noun contained the same gender assignment cue as the target word. For instance, Flagge F instead of Fahne F ‘flag’ was included because both contain the schwa-cue but Bodensee M ‘Lake of Constance’ instead of Strand M ‘beach’ was excluded.

If children could not name the depicted item themselves, the answer was not included. The rationale for this is that an unknown word is like a nonce word for the child, and we wanted to restrict the results of experiment 1 to words that the children knew.

We included non-target-like productions in which the children added an ‘s’ or ‘r’ to the adjective to make the adjective rhyme with the definite determiner in the neuter and masculine conditions, e.g., das N   *rote-s Bett N (‘the red bed’) , der M   *grosse-r Schrank M (‘the big closet’). Although the adjective form is not target-like, it can be taken to indicate the assigned gender.

We coded inconsistently marked gender across the three elicited responses and agreement mismatches within the DP, e.g., eine F grosses N Bett N (‘a big bed’). In both cases, it is not clear which gender the children have assigned, and these responses were not included when determining which gender the children had assigned.

Finally, we excluded data from nine children who completed less than 50% of the task or provided responses without determiners or any gender-marked adjective more than 50% of the time, two from mixed families and several from Russian families (see Table 3 ).

4.4 Results

The remaining dataset consisted of 44 bilingual German-Russian children, 16 from German-Russian families (age 3–11; mean 6,62; sd = 2.40) and 28 from Russian families (ages 4–10, mean 6.51; sd = 1,78). Table 5 shows that the children from Russian families omitted determiners most often. Gender switches across DPs were infrequent in all groups.

Children producing no gender marked forms and inconsistent gender marking.

As a next step, we excluded children who defaulted to one particular gender. Defaulting was defined as using one gender 75% to 100% of the time. There were no monolingual defaulters, but among the bilinguals five defaulted to feminine and one to neuter. Discarding defaulters left us with 23 monolinguals and 38 bilinguals (23 from RR families and 15 from mixed families). The results of the Real word task are illustrated in Figure 2 for the feminine condition (left), the masculine condition (middle) and the neuter condition (right) for each of the three groups of children. Arrows indicate where we find significant differences.

Figure 2

Gender assignment with real nouns in German: monolingual children (GG), children from German-Russian (GR) and children from Russian-Russian (RR) families. Significant contrasts between the groups are marked by arrows.

We applied generalized linear mixed effects logistic regression analysis to statistically model the probability of an accurate response on the real word task depending on the three main predictors (Family type, Condition and Age) and their interactions. Model comparison (Anova) indicated that Age (continuous) as a main effect and the interaction of Family and Age could be dropped. The final model included the main effects of Family type and Condition, their interaction, as well as an interaction of Age and Condition, and a three-way interaction of Condition, Family and Age. Participants and Items were included as random intercepts. All models were fit using R version 4.0.3 (release 2020-10-10). The analysis revealed significant interactions of Age and Condition (masculine p = .03, and neuter p = .02), indicating that all groups of participants become significantly more accurate with age with respect to masculine and neuter gender as compared to the feminine. The output of the final model is summarized in Table A1 in the Appendix. For a more detailed comparison of the groups within conditions, we conducted post-hoc pairwise comparisons using the R package emmeans ( Lenth et al. 2019 ). These revealed that the monolingual children performed significantly better than the children from Russian (RR) and mixed families (GR) on the masculine ( p < .001 in both cases) and neuter conditions ( p = .003 and p < .001, respectively). No other contrasts were significant (see Tables A2 in Appendix 1).

Overall, the results indicate that children in all groups use all three genders productively (above 60% accuracy with all genders), although nouns in the feminine condition seem to be the least difficult.

5. Experiment 2 (Nonce words)

The purpose of Experiment 2 was to see how children performed with nonce nouns that contained the same gender cues as the real nouns in Experiment 1. The Nonce word task preceded the Real word task and was carried out on a different day.

5.1 Participants

The participants were the same as in Experiment 1, but more children took this task. The initial dataset consisted of 60 bilingual German-Russian children, 20 from German-Russian families (age 3–11; mean 6.67; sd = 2.47) and 40 from Russian families (age 4–10 years, mean 6.50; sd = 1.89). We tested the same 23 monolingual German children as in Experiment 1.

Inspired by Karmiloff-Smith’s ( 1979 ) Experiment 8, we created a task with 24 nonce words (8 per gender category), e.g., Schromp (M) , Fruxe (F) , Knett (N) , which followed the same assignment rules as those in Experiment 1 (see Appendix 2). We ensured that these nonce nouns formed no minimal pairs with existing words in German and piloted them with monolingual German adults. The adults assigned the predicted gender between 70% and 100% of the time (71% M; 80% N; 88% F).

As in Experiment 1, the children were told a story about Martians, but this time they had brought several objects that were non-existent on earth. When leaving their spacecraft to explore the area, a thief broke into their spacecraft, and the children were asked to help him carry the bag with stolen items. The children were given a small bag into which they could place the items (pictured on small cards). The items were introduced to the children by means of a PowerPoint presentation (with drawings of imaginary objects taken from the Horst & Hout 2016 noun database). They were non-animate objects of different shapes. When the items were introduced, special care was taken not to reveal the targeted gender. Again, two indefinite DPs and one definite DP were elicited (see (2), relevant DPs in bold).

  • Example procedure for Experiment 2
  • Situation: Children are shown a yellow Knett and a blue Knett.
  •        Was du hier siehst heißt Knett. Was ist das?
  •        (What you see here is called Knett (N) . What do you see?)
  • Ein gelbes (Knett) und ein blaues Knett .
  • (a N yellow N (Knett) and a N blue N Knett)
  • Situation: Animated appearance of the thief. The blue Knett disappears, while the yellow Knett remains visible.
  •        Was klaut der Dieb?
  •        (What’s the thief stealing?)
  • Das blaue Knett .
  • (the N blue Knett)

There were three practice items whose gender was predictable based on suffixes (one associated with each gender) that were not used in the experiment. If necessary, the practice session was repeated until the child understood the task.

5.3 Analysis

Tables 6 provides an overview of the responses for the monolingual and bilingual groups. Again, masculine and neuter responses cannot be distinguished in the indefinite condition due to syncretism.

Nonce word experiment, overview of responses (predominant answer pattern in bold).

The coding of responses followed the same algorithm as in Experiment 1 (see above) with some adjustments for nonce nouns. We discarded the following types of items:

Cases in which the child did not (re)produce the target word, unless the cue was kept constant (e.g., Klapett (N) for Lapett (N) was included, but Fretts (?) for Fruxe (F) was excluded);

Instances in which it was impossible to determine the assigned gender, (i) because the gender was incongruent between the determiner and the noun, e.g., * eine F blaues N Plett , (ii) because the gender was underspecified, e.g., blau Plett (no article, no inflection on the adjective), (iii) because the children switched genders across the elicited DPs.

As before, we included only children who completed more than 50% of the task. The resulting dataset consisted of 23 monolingual German-speaking children, 44 bilingual German-Russian children: 18 from German-Russian families and 26 from Russian families.

5.4 Results

Similar to the Real word task, the groups differed significantly in the probability of providing an article-less response: children from Russian families did so 32% of the time, while children from German-Russian families did so only 15%. Again, the children rarely switched (only 4% of the time with no differences between children from the two family types). The monolinguals failed to provide an article or another gender-marked element only 6% of the time, while gender switches constituted 1% of the data. When coding the data, we proceeded as follows, in analogy to Experiment 1.

The assigned gender was coded based on the definite article.

If no article was produced in the definite condition, we used the indefinite condition as an indicator of the assigned gender: adjectives marked as masculine (e.g., roter ) or neuter (e.g., rotes ) were taken to be indicators of the assigned gender. Recall that in the absence of an article, feminine marked adjectives (e.g., rote ) were not considered, because the form can represent all genders in definite DPs (cf. Table 1 and footnote 4).

We included non-target-like productions in which the children added an ‘s’ or ‘r’ to the adjective to make the adjective rhyme with the definite determiner in the neuter and masculine conditions, e.g., das N   *rote-s Plett N (‘the red bed’).

We excluded cases in which gender was marked inconsistently across the three elicited responses and cases of agreement mismatches within the DP, e.g., eine F grosses N Plett N (‘a big Plett’).

As a result of the first cleaning, we excluded cases where the child did not respond, produced a noun that was different from the prompted noun, a bare noun and a bare adjective, an incomprehensible response or one that couldn’t be classified as either M, F or N. The remaining data were classified into M, F and N responses following the procedure described in 5.3 above. Figure 3 illustrates the distribution of responses in the Nonce word experiment.

Figure 3

Gender assignment with nonce nouns in German: monolingual children (GG), children from German-Russian (GR) and children from Russian-Russian (RR) families. Significant contrasts between groups are marked by arrows.

To statistically model the probability of following the phonological gender cue in the Nonce word task, we fit a series of generalized linear mixed effects logistic regressions with different sets of predictors. The best model included three main effects: Condition, Family type, and Age (continuous), two two-way interactions (Condition and Family, and Condition and Age), and a three-way interaction of Condition, Family and Age. An Anova model comparison suggested that the interaction of Family and Age could be dropped. Participants and Items were included as random intercepts. The model revealed a significant effect of Family (RR: p = .007), Condition (Masculine: p = .008, Neuter: p < .001) and Age ( p = .002), as well as a significant interaction of Condition (N) and Family (DR: p = .03; RR: p < .001), a significant interaction of Condition (N) and Age ( p < .001) and four significant three-way interactions of Condition (F), Age and Family (DR: p = .004; RR: p < .001), and Condition (N), Age and Family (DR: p = .003; RR: p = .001). The output of the model is summarized in Table B1 in the Appendix.

Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of groups within conditions revealed three significant differences. First, the GR and RR children were significantly more likely than the GG children to use Feminine gender in the Feminine condition ( p < .001). GR children were marginally but not significantly different from the RR children ( p = .07). Second, RR children were significantly more likely to use Masculine in the Masculine condition than GG children ( p = .01). GR children were marginally different from the GG children ( p = .07). Third, GG children were significantly more likely to use Neuter in the Neuter condition than both the GR and the RR children ( p < .001 in both cases). No other differences were significant. The results of the post-hoc comparisons are summarized in Table B2 in the Appendix.

It should be noted that many children chose one particular gender (almost) throughout the entire task. We had a closer look at these defaulters (classified as children who used one particular gender 75% of the time or more) and non-defaulters separately. Among the 23 monolinguals, 11 children used neuter 75% or more of the time, and one child (3 years old) used feminine 100% of the time. The bilingual data varies more in terms of default strategies. Among the 44 bilinguals, 13 defaulted to neuter and 8 to feminine. The latter are distributed equally over German-Russian and Russian families. Interestingly, defaulting correlates with age, as younger children default less to neuter (all 3-year-olds are non-neuter-defaulters, while most 6-year-olds and all 7-year-olds are neuter-defaulters). Table 7 provides an overview of defaulting strategies comparing mono- and bilinguals. The distribution of (non-)defaulters and their demographics is presented in Table 8 , indicating that as many as half of the children in each group chose a default strategy in their responses. Interestingly, the vast majority of monolingual defaulters used Neuter gender across the board, while bilingual defaulters fall into two groups: children that used Feminine across the board and participants that defaulted to Neuter. There were no Masculine defaulters in the dataset.

Default strategies in mono- and bilinguals.

Distribution of non-defaulters in Experiment 2.

Figure 4 presents a comparison of responses by condition in the monolinguals and the two groups of bilinguals (excluding defaulters).

Figure 4

Gender response per condition: Monolinguals (GR) vs. Bilinguals from German-Russian (GR) and Russian (RR) families (excluding defaulters). Significant contrasts between the groups are marked by arrows.

We used a similar analysis as described above to assess the effect of the main predictors (Family type, Condition and Age) on the probability of following the gender cue provided in the Nonce word for a subset of children who did not default to one gender throughout the experiment. The winning model included the same predictors as the winning model for the whole Nonce word dataset, i.e., the three main effects: Condition, Family type, and Age, two two-way interactions (Condition and Family, and Condition and Age), and a three-way interaction of Condition, Family and Age. Participants and Items were included as random intercepts. The model revealed a significant effect of Condition (Masculine: p = .001 and Neuter: p = .004), as well as a significant interaction of Condition (Neuter) and Age ( p = .002) and two significant three-way interaction of Condition (Neuter), Age and Family (GR: p = .04 and RR: p = .02). The output of the model is summarized in Table B3 in the Appendix.

Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of groups within conditions revealed the following significant differences. First, the GR and RR children were significantly more likely than the GG children to use Feminine gender in the Feminine condition ( p < .001 in both cases). Second, RR children were significantly more likely to use Masculine in the Masculine condition than the GG children ( p = .03). Third, the GG and GR children were significantly more likely to use Neuter in the Neuter condition than the RR children ( p = .005 and p = .03, respectively). No other differences were significant. The results of the post-hoc comparisons are summarized in Table B4 in the Appendix.

The results indicate that after the exclusion of the defaulters both bilingual groups exhibit a significantly higher sensitivity to the feminine gender cue than the monolinguals, and the RR children exhibit a higher sensitivity to the masculine cue. In the neuter condition, the monolinguals and children from mixed families produce more neuter responses than children from Russian families. The children from Russian families use more masculine in the neuter condition, and it is possible that they are delayed in separating the masculine vs. neuter categories, as has been reported for monolinguals in the literature (cf. section 2.2).

6. Experiment 3 (mixed cues)

The purpose of Experiment 3 was to see whether children pay more attention to phonological or to structural (agreement) cues when assigning gender to nonce nouns. Since sensitivity to structural cues as opposed to phonological cues is known to increase with age in languages with relatively transparent assignment systems, such as French (e.g., Karmiloff-Smith 1979 ) and Russian ( Rodina 2008 ; Rodina & Westergaard, 2012 ), the same might hold for German. Unlike in Experiment 2, we provided a gender cue when introducing the nonce noun. Experiment 3 followed Experiment 2 but was carried out on a different day.

We tested 20 monolinguals (ages 3–9, mean 5) and 38 bilinguals (age 4–10, mean 7), 11 from GR families and 27 from RR families. The bilingual children were a subset of the children who participated in Experiments 1 and 2.

The experiment was inspired by Karmiloff-Smith’s ( 1979 ) Experiment 9 and involved 18 stimuli. The nouns were taken from a larger pool of items than in Experiment 2. A third of the stimuli (N = 6) contained DPs in which the gender cue on the noun was consistent (matched) with that on the article and adjective (e.g., eine F   blaue F   Fruxe (F) ); the remainder (N =12) contained DPs with conflicting (mismatching) gender cues on article+adjective vs. noun ending (e.g., eine F   blaue F   Glett (N) ). The stimuli were balanced for gender (see Appendix 2 for an overview of the stimuli).

The children were told that the Martians were on another trip exploring Earth, and while they were gone, the thief broke into their spacecraft again. The experimenter invited the children to look at what he stole this time. This time, one DP with cues was provided and only two additional DPs were elicited. Note that in the mixed cue condition, the child may repeat the gender markings present in the adult utterance, although they contain a mismatch between the structural and the phonological cue, or they may change the article and adjective (i.e., the structural cues) to match them with the noun ending (i.e., the phonological cue).

  • Example procedure in Experiment 3, children see a blue Glett
  • Experimenter: Das auf meiner Seite ist eine F blaue F Glett (N) . Und auf deiner Seite?
  •                                (This here on my side is a blue Glett. And on your side?)
  • Exp. Answer:
  •      Eine F   rote F   Glett (N) /ein M/N   rotes N   Glett (N) .
  •     (a red Glett/a red Glett)
  •   Und was ist weg?
  •   (And what is gone?)
  •      Die F   rote Glett (N) /das N   rote Glett N .
  •     (the red Glett/the red Glett)

6.3 Analysis

We followed a similar cleaning procedure as in Experiments 1 and 2 and discarded responses where the child did not produce the target word or where it was impossible to determine the assigned gender due to incongruent gender markings or underspecified gender. Unlike in Experiment 2, where no structural gender cues were provided, we did not observe any strong defaulting patterns. Only one bilingual child used feminine 100% of the time, and there were no neuter or masculine defaulters. Five further children (all from Russian families) were excluded from the analysis, because they produced less than 50% codable responses. The resulting dataset included 20 German monolingual children, 11 children from German-Russian families and 21 children from Russian families. No significant differences were observed between the bilingual children from German-Russian and Russian families, which motivated analyzing their results together. Unlike in the other experiments, children switched genders more often between their two responses. Monolinguals switched genders 37% of the time, children from German-Russian families 25% of the time and children from Russian families 28% of the time. As much as 80% (72/89) of all switches in the monolingual group were from M or F to N (consistent with the defaulting strategy). Bilinguals switched to N less than 50% of the time. The higher number of switches in this experiment was expected because the children were provided with conflicting cues.

6.4 Results

When the cues on the two targets were consistent with those on the noun (match condition), both mono- and bilingual children mostly produced a definite DP with the same gender as the one provided in the prompt, in fact as much as 85% of the time. Figure 5 summarizes the proportions of responses where children used the same gender as the one provided by the experimenter in the Match condition. 5

Figure 5

Proportion of responses following the prompted (structural) gender cues in the Match condition by age.

As evident from Figure 5 , both groups of children exhibited high sensitivity to the prompted gender cues. At the same time, younger children (especially bilinguals) produced a higher proportion of responses that did not follow the gender cue provided by the experimenter.

The main research question of Experiment 3 was whether we would observe any effect of group or age in the probability of following either the structural cue (on the determiner and adjective) or the phonological gender cue (on the noun ending) provided in the Mismatch condition. Figures 6 and 7 illustrate the proportions of responses that followed the adjectival or the phonological cue by individual participants from the two groups.

Figure 6

Proportion of responses following the structural cue in the Mismatch condition by age.

Figure 7

Proportion of responses following the phonological cue in the Mismatch condition by age.

Figures 6 and 7 indicate that monolinguals have a higher probability of following the structural gender cue in their responses than the bilinguals. In both groups, the probability of following the structural (and not the phonological cue) increases with age, and this increase is more visible in the bilingual group.

To analyze the results statistically, we applied generalized linear mixed effects logistic regression analysis where the probability of following the structural cue or the phonological cue was modelled based on the effects of Group (Bilingual vs Monolingual), Condition (Match vs Mismatch) and Age (continuous), as well as their interactions. An Anova model comparison was applied to choose the best model for each dependent variable. The final model predicting the probability of following the structural cue included three main fixed effects, an interaction of Group and Age, and a three-way interaction of all predictors. Participants and Items were included as random intercepts. The model revealed a marginally significant effect of group ( p = .05), and a significant effect of age ( p < .001), indicating that both groups become more likely to follow the structural gender cue with age, and that the monolinguals are marginally more likely to follow this cue than the bilinguals. No other effects were significant. The output of the model is summarized in Table C1 in the Appendix. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of groups within conditions indicated that the monolinguals were significantly different from the bilinguals in the mismatch condition ( p = .02), while there were no differences in the match condition (see Table C2 in the Appendix).

The model that predicted the probability of following the phonological cue included three main fixed effects, two interactions: Group and Condition, and Group and Age, and a three-way interaction of all predictors. Participants and Items were included as random intercepts. The model revealed a significant effect of Group ( p = .03), and a significant effect of Age ( p < .001), indicating that both groups become less likely to follow the phonological gender cue with age, and that the bilinguals were generally more likely to follow the phonological gender cue than the monolinguals. Furthermore, the model revealed a significant interaction of Group and Condition ( p = .007), and a significant three-way-interaction of Group, Condition and Age ( p < .001). No other effects were significant. The output of the model is summarized in Table C3 in the Appendix. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of groups within conditions indicated that the monolinguals were again significantly different from the bilinguals in the mismatch condition ( p = .02), while there were no differences in the match condition.

7. Discussion

We have investigated whether German-learning children are sensitive to phonological gender cues and which default strategies they use ( RQ1 ). We expected both mono- and bilingual children to be sensitive to phonological gender cues and we expected this sensitivity to be dependent on cue reliability; we further expected defaulting to masculine. We explored potential quantitative and/or qualitative differences between monolinguals and bilinguals and asked whether those could be related to CLI from Russian ( RQ2 ). We expected a facilitative effect of Russian and higher sensitivity to phonological cues in bilinguals compared to monolinguals, especially with nonce words. We finally investigated the assignment strategies German-speaking mono- and bilinguals prefer by exposing them to conflicting cues, i.e., structural vs. phonological ( RQ3 ). We expected a higher sensitivity to phonological cues in younger children and to structural cues in older children.

7.1 Sensitivity to formal cues in German vs. lexical learning

Experiment 1 showed that with familiar nouns, mono- and bilingual children acquiring German assign gender successfully in all three gender categories. Both groups of children were most accurate with the feminine nouns and least accurate with the masculine nouns. Given that most neuter nouns in our experiment had a very low frequency too, this suggests that factors beyond the frequency of noun items are at play. We hypothesize that form-based regularities connected to the phonological endings of the nouns are implicated: children showed at-ceiling accuracy with the feminine schwa-final nouns, where cue reliability is the highest, followed by the neuter nouns ending in - ett . The reliability of the masculine cue was very low, and accordingly, the children’s accuracy on this condition was the lowest. There were quantitative differences between the monolingual and the bilingual children, which can be expected given that the bilinguals, especially those from Russian families, had had less exposure to German.

Despite a general sensitivity to cues, the comparison between Experiments 1 and 2 shows that both groups are considerably less successful when assigning gender to novel words where only a phonological cue on the noun is provided. This finding is in line with the proposal that there exist two routes of grammatical gender processing: the lexical route, which retrieves grammatical gender features of the noun directly from the lexicon, and a form-based route that relies on form-based properties of the grammatical gender classes (see Caffarra et al. 2014 ). While the gender features of real words are stored in the lexicon and are deterministic, the form-based cues only reflect probabilistic tendencies and may differ in their reliability. The results of our Nonce word task may further reflect the underlying differences in reliability between different types of form-based gender cues. The feminine gender cue (final -e ) was by far the most reliable cue for the participants, followed by the neuter cue, and finally the masculine cue. This mirrors the results for the adults when we piloted the nonce words.

Overall, phonological gender cues seem to have a relatively low reliability in German. In both groups, many children resort to a default gender in Experiment 2, choosing one particular gender 75% of the time or more. At the same time, children who do not default show a certain sensitivity to the respective gender cues. Those monolingual children who do not default to neuter assign neuter to nouns with the neuter cue, use feminine gender most often when the phonological cue is feminine, and masculine gender when the phonological cue is masculine. This means that defaulting and sensitivity to cues and regularities do not exclude one another, as has also been found by Ruberg ( 2013 ; see Enger 2009 for similar observations in adult systems). The picture for the bilingual children is less clear because some children default to neuter and others to feminine. More specifically, children from Russian families with less German input are more likely to default to feminine than children from mixed families with more exposure to German (the latter, by defaulting to neuter, behave more akin to German monolinguals). The non-defaulters use all three genders, with feminine being most frequent in the feminine condition, masculine in the masculine condition, and neuter in the neuter condition; the division between masculine vs. neuter is less clear, as shown in many previous studies on German (e.g., Stern & Stern 1928 ; Mills 1986 ; Müller 1999 ; Dieser 2009 ; Hopp 2011 ).

Finally, a comparison of Experiment 2 and the matching cue condition in Experiment 3 shows that mono- and bilingual children are more successful at assigning gender if there is a structural cue on a gender target (article or adjective) in addition to a phonological cue on the noun. Indeed, the defaulting patterns observed in Experiment 2 were not replicated in Experiment 3, where the children received structural cues in addition to phonological cues. This may be taken to indicate that children are sensitive to the relevant cues, but that phonological cues in German are weaker than in other languages.

7.2 Differences between monolinguals and bilinguals

As outlined above, both mono- and bilingual children have been found to be sensitive to phonological and structural gender cues in German. Based on previous research ( Kaltsa et al. 2019 ; Egger et al. 2018 ; van Dijk et al. 2021 ) we hypothesized that bilingual German-Russian children may experience ‘acceleration’ (or facilitation) in the acquisition of gender in German due CLI from their heritage language Russian. More specifically, bilinguals were expected to be more sensitive to gender regularities in German, due to the high transparency of gender assignment in Russian. This was indeed borne out, as the bilinguals performed better than the monolinguals on both the feminine and the masculine cue in the nonce word task, i.e., nouns ending in schwa and nouns ending in a consonant (Experiment 2, see Figure 4 ). However, they did not perform better than the monolinguals on the neuter cue (which also ends in a consonant). This could be because they treat all consonant-final nouns as belonging to the same noun class (as in Russian) and thus use more masculine gender in this context. Alternatively (or additionally), the high performance of the monolinguals on the neuter cue is not due to sensitivity to the cue, but rather the tendency of the monolinguals to default to neuter.

One major difference between monolinguals and bilinguals concerns their defaulting strategies (N in monolinguals and F or N in bilinguals). First of all, the defaulting patterns observed in our study are somewhat unexpected given previous claims about masculine being the default for gender assignment in German (e.g., Steinmetz 2006 ; Ruberg 2013 ) and neuter being late acquired ( Müller 1999 ; Eichler et al. 2013 ). Interestingly, among the monolingual children, the neuter defaulters represent the older children. It thus seems as if the F and M cues on the nouns were to some degree available to younger children, but as they grow older, the phonological cues get overridden by a more powerful neuter default. There are three possible explanations for this: (i) children identify the nonce objects with things, and the word for thing ( Ding ) is neuter in German (furthermore, according to Zubin & Köpcke 1986 , nouns with a ‘vague’ reference, e.g., Objekt ‘object’, Zeug ‘stuff’, and nouns denoting superordinates, e.g., Instrument ‘instrument’, Möbel ‘furniture’, are generally Neuter in German, although some of these nouns may not be frequent in colloquial and child speech), (ii) the children were primed by the relative pronoun and question word was , because it ends in – s , which is a typical ending for neuter gender targets, 6 viz. the definite article das and inflected adjectives, such as rotes ‘red’, or (iii) children default to neuter gender because it is in fact the default gender in German (cf. Köpcke & Zubin 2009 , see section 2.1). Alternatively, the children are affected by the syntactic default, a possibility which gains plausibility given the challenges pertaining to German gender: if children postulate phonological rules, they must discover that there are many exceptions to them, while there are no exceptions to syntactic rules of gender agreement. On the other hand, there is no evidence, so far, that syntactic defaults are transferred to function as morphological default. Clearly, more research is needed to investigate the issue of grammatical gender default in German. We leave this question for future research.

We further suspect that the bilinguals’ defaulting to feminine has different reasons than the monolinguals’ defaulting to neuter, and that it could be motivated phonologically. The feminine cue is the strongest for the bilinguals, since it is the category with the highest percentage of target-like responses. This makes sense because the phonological cue for feminine is not only present in noun endings, but also on articles and adjectives, e.g., ein e   klein e   Kerz e ‘a small candle’, and the adjective form reappears with all genders in definite DPs ( der M   gross e   X, die F   gross e   X, das N   gross e   X ‘the big X’). Thus, children hear determiners, adjectives and nouns ending in schwa very frequently (see also Mills 1986 ), and by using more feminine overall, children might prime themselves. That is, the more they hear these forms, the more they produce them. An alternative (or additional) explanation is the existence of a similar cue in Russian, viz. nouns ending in schwa, which is mainly feminine (cf. section 2.3). While we think it is unlikely that the bilinguals link feminine gender in Russian to feminine gender in German, they may simply be more sensitive to this phonological cue in German, knowing that a similar cue is important (and highly predictive) in their heritage language.

Another difference between monolinguals and bilinguals is that the bilingual children seem to be more sensitive to the masculine cue. This is particularly evident in the group of children from Russian families. Given that consonantal endings in Russian are associated with masculine gender, it is not unlikely that children have used their knowledge of Russian cues and applied it to German, where our masculine nouns ended in a consonant. In fact, some children do not seem to distinguish clearly between masculine and neuter, and this can be expected because the neuter nouns in Experiment 2 ended in a consonant too. Note that transfer of cues, as we have speculated here, would not imply that the bilingual children make a connection between masculine, feminine, and neuter gender in German and the corresponding genders in Russian. Rather, since we know that they are highly sensitive to the consonantal noun-ending cue in Russian, they may simply assume that ending in a consonant is a transparent gender cue also in German, and thus expect that all nouns in -C will belong to the same gender . This means that it is not important which gender this is. The bilingual children may select the M gender forms simply because they are more frequent than N in German. Under this scenario, the ‘transfer’ of a cue would be based on the phonological feature and unrelated to a lexical feature.

In conclusion, mono- and bilingual children are sensitive to cues, but monolingual children default to neuter, while bilingual children default to neuter or feminine. It is possible that this is merely an effect of exposure and age, since the bilingual children have had less cumulative exposure to German, and it might be that the feminine defaulters would have picked a different strategy, had they been older. Admittedly, our study covers a large age range and the bilingual group is heterogeneous in terms of the learning profiles in German, specifically age of onset and exposure, which makes further research with larger participant groups desirable. At the same time, we were interested in the developmental patterns implicated in the acquisition of gender in German, thus looking at a wider age range was necessary in our study.

7.3 Phonological vs. structural cues

The results of Experiment 3 show that all groups of children rely more on structural cues than on phonological cues in a situation of cue mismatch. For the bilingual children we observed a significant developmental trend, with their reliance on phonological cues significantly decreasing and reliance on syntactic cues significantly increasing with age, in line with previous findings from languages with more transparent phonological gender rules ( Karmiloff-Smith 1979 , Lidz & Gagliardi 2015 ). The reason why we see relatively little sensitivity to phonological cues (unlike what Karmiloff-Smith ( 1979 ) has shown for French) might indeed be that these are less reliable in German, and that children start paying attention to structural cues from an earlier age, i.e., once they discover that there are numerous exceptions to phonological regularities.

8. Conclusions

We have investigated sensitivity to phonological and structural cues to gender marking in monolingual German and Russian-German bilingual children. The results show that both groups of children use phonological cues on the nouns to assign gender, but they are significantly better at assigning gender to existing and familiar nouns exhibiting the same cues. This suggests that cue sensitivity takes some time to develop, but it may also be the case that retrieval (of a familiar noun and its gender) is easier and faster than computation (assigning gender to a noun that is not in the mental lexicon). In addition, many children default to one specific gender, with mono- and bilinguals displaying different defaulting strategies. Children are more successful at assigning gender and refrain from defaulting when structural cues are provided, suggesting that using phonological and structural cues is an available and important strategy even in the acquisition of German, where phonological cues are less reliable than in many other languages. There is a trend for the bilingual children, in particular those with considerable exposure to Russian, to rely more on phonological cues than the monolinguals. This was especially visible in the masculine condition of Experiment 2, which tested gender assignment with nonce nouns, and also to some extent in the feminine. This finding suggests that bilingual children may transfer their knowledge of cues for grammatical gender from one language to another, at least if the other language has more reliable phonological cues, which was the case in our study. This indicates that the bilingual children in our study did not transfer the lexical gender of individual nouns but show general sensitivity to phonological cues on a more abstract level. Thus, our study contributes to the literature on CLI in bilingualism. Another interesting finding is that neither monolingual nor bilingual children defaulted to masculine gender in German, although masculine is the suggested default in the theoretical literature. This raises new questions about the definition of defaults, which should be investigated in future research.

Additional Files

The additional files for this article can be found as follows:

Details of the Statistical Analysis. DOI: https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5696.s1

Experimental Stimuli. DOI: https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5696.s2

  • This type of transfer would correspond to Sabourin, Stowe & de Haan’s ( 2006 ) notion of “surface transfer”. [ ^ ]
  • There is more to be said about semantic regularities, but since we did not test them in this paper, we refrain from discussing them in more detail. [ ^ ]
  • The relatively large age range and varying AoO also means that total length of exposure varied. However, amount of exposure to German also varied greatly so that in some cases children with shorter exposure were more fluent than children with longer exposure. Thus, neither AoO nor length of exposure were necessarily indicative of the children’s proficiencies, especially amongst the younger children. We therefore included all children, but those whose proficiency was too low were filtered out if they could not respond to the task (see description of experimental procedures below). [ ^ ]
  • In other words, we cannot be sure whether a bare noun produced in the indefinite condition was intended to be definite, in which case the feminine-looking adjective form could potentially represent all genders. [ ^ ]
  • The highest number of non-cued gender responses occurred in the masculine condition (37N, 75F = 28%) and the lowest in the feminine condition (4M, 19N = 6%) (neuter condition: 22M, 35F = 15%). When the children assigned a gender that was not cued at all, they used all three genders, but bilingual children tended to use more feminine and monolingual children more neuter, mirroring their defaulting-preferences in Experiment 2. [ ^ ]
  • The relative pronoun was occurs in the absence of an antecedent with specified gender and number features. [ ^ ]

Acknowledgements

We wish to thanks Simone Waitz and Sarah Zander for their invaluable help during data collection and coding. This research was supported by a grant from the Research Council of Norway for the project Microvariation in Multilingual Acquisition & Attrition Situations (MiMS), project code 250857. The paper was completed while most authors were fellows at the Centre for Advanced Study at the Norwegian Academy of Science and Letters in Oslo during the academic year 2019–2020, working on the research project MultiGender.

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Binanzer, Anja. 2017. Genus–Kongruenz und Klassifikation: Evidenzen aus dem Zweitspracherwerb des Deutschen (Bd. 17). Berlin/Boston: Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co KG. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110548549

Bittner, Dagmar. 2006. Case before gender in the acquisition of German. Folia Linguistica 40(1–2). 115–134. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/flin.40.1part2.115

Bottari, Piero & Cipriani, Paola & Chilosi, Anna Maria. 1993/1994. Protosyntactic devices in the acquisition of Italian free morphology. Language Acquisition 3(4). 327–369. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0304_1

Caffarra, Sendy & Janssen, Niels & Barber, Horacio A. 2014. Two sides of gender: ERP evidence for the presence of two routes during gender agreement processing. Neuropsychologia 63. 124–34. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.016

Ceitlin, Stella N. 2009. Ocherki po slovoobrazovaniju i formoobrazovaniju v detskoj rechi . [ On Inflection and Derivation in Child Language ]. Moscow: Znak.

Corbett, Greville. 1982. Gender in Russian: An account of gender specification and its relationship to declension. Russian Linguistics 6. 197–232. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1007/BF03545848

Corbett, Greville. 1991. Gender . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Corbett, Greville & Fraser, Norman. 2000. Default genders. In Unterbeck, Barbara & Rissanen, Matti (eds.), Gender in Grammar and Cognition , 55–97. Berlin: De Gruyter. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110802603.55

Cornips, Leonie & Hulk, Aafke. 2008. Factors of success and failure in the acquisition of grammatical gender in Dutch. Second Language Research 24(3). 267–295. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1177/0267658308090182

Corteen, Emma C. 2019. The assignment of grammatical gender in German: Testing Optimal Gender Assignment Theory . University of Cambridge dissertation.

Dieser, Elena. 2009. Genuserwerb im Russischen und Deutschen: Korpusgestützte Studie zu ein-und zweisprachigen Kindern und Erwachsenen . München/Berlin: Sagner. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.3726/b12030

Egger, Evelyn & Hulk, Aafke & Tsimpli, Ianthi Maria. 2018. Crosslinguistic influence in the discovery of gender: the case of Greek–Dutch bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21(4). 694–709. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000207

Eichler, Nadine & Jansen, Veronika & Müller, Natascha. 2013. Gender acquisition in bilingual children: French–German, Italian–German, Spanish–German and Italian–French. International Journal of Bilingualism 17(5). 550–572. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911435719

Enger, Hans-Olav. 2009. The role of core and non-core semantic rules in gender assignment. Lingua 119(9). 1281–1299. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2009.02.004

Hopp, Holger. 2011. Internal and external factors in the child L2 development of the German determiner Phrase. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1(3). 238–264. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.02hop

Horst, Jessica S. & Hout, Michael C. 2016. The Novel object and unusual name (NOUN) database: a collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behavioral Research Method 48(4). 1393–1409. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0647-3

Hulk, Aafke & van der Linden, Elisabeth 2010. How vulnerable is gender? In Guijarro-Fuentes, Pedro & Domiínguez, Laura (eds.), New Directions in Language Acquisition: Romance Languages in the Generative Perspective , 107–134. Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

Kaltsa, Maria & Tsimpli, Ianthi Maria & Argyri, Froso. 2019. The development of gender assignment and agreement in English-Greek and German-Greek bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9(2). 253–288. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.16033.kal

Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1979. A functional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Klassert, Annegret & Gagarina, Natalia & Kauschke, Christina. 2014. Object and action naming in Russian-and German-speaking monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17(1). 73–88. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000096

Köpcke, Klaus-Michael. 1982. Zum Genussystem der deutschen Gegenwartssprache . Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/9783111676562

Köpcke, Klaus-Michael & Zubin, David. 1983. Die kognitive Organisation der Genuszuweisung zu den einsilbigen Nomen der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Zeitschrift für germanistische Linguistik 11. 166–182. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/zfgl.1983.11.2.166

Köpcke, Klaus-Michael & Zubin, David. 2009. Genus. In Hentschel, Elke & Vogel, Petra M. (eds.) Deutsche Morphologie , 132–154. Berlin: de Gruyter.

Kupisch, Tanja & Müller, Natascha & Cantone, Katja Francesca. 2002. Gender in monolingual and bilingual first language acquisition: Comparing Italian and French. Lingue e Linguaggio 1. 107–150.

Lemmerth, Natalia & Hopp, Holger. 2018. Gender processing in simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Cross-linguistic lexical and syntactic influences. Language Acquisition 26(1). 21–45. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2017.1391815

Lenth, Russell & Singman, Henrik & Love, Jonathon & Buerkner, Paul & Herve, Maxime. 2019. R package emmeans : Estimated Marginal means, aka Least-Squares Means. R repository: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans/index.html

Lidz, Jeffrey & Gagliardi, Annie. 2015. How Nature Meets Nurture: Universal Grammar and Statistical Learning. Annual Review of Linguistics 1(1). 333–353. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-125236

Mills, Anne E. 1986. The Acquisition of Gender. A Study of English and German . Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1986.24.1.31

Mitrofanova, Natalia & Rodina, Yulia, & Urek, Olga & Westergaard, Marit. 2018. Bilinguals’ sensitivity to grammatical gender cues in Russian: the role of cumulative input, proficiency, and dominance. Frontiers in Psychology 9. 1894. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01894

Mitrofanova, Natalia & Urek, Olga & Rodina, Yulia & Westergaard, Marit. 2021. Sensitivity to microvariation in bilingual acquisition: morphophonological gender cues in Russian heritage language. Applied Psycholinguistics , 1–39. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716421000382

Montanari, Elke G. & Abel, Roman & Graßer, Barbara & Tschudinovski, Lilia. 2018. Do bilinguals create two different sets of vocabulary for two domains? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8(4). 502–522. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.16021.mon

Müller, Natascha. 1999. Gender and number in acquisition. In Unterbeck, Barbara & Rissanen, Matti (eds.), Gender in grammar and cognition: Approaches to Gender , 351–399. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110802603.351

Opitz, Andreas & Thomas, Pechmann. 2016. Gender features in German. Evidence for underspecification. The Mental Lexicon 11(2). 216–241. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1075/ml.11.2.03opi

Pérez-Pereira, Miguel. 1991. The acquisition of gender: what Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language 18(3). 571–590. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011259

Polinsky, Maria. 2008. Gender under incomplete acquisition: heritage speakers’ knowledge of noun categorization. Heritage Language Journal 6. 40–71. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.46538/hlj.6.1.3

Rodina, Yulia. 2008. Semantics and Morphology: The Acquisition of Grammatical Gender in Russian . University of Tromsø dissertation.

Rodina, Yulia & Westergaard, Marit. 2012. A cue-based approach to the acquisition of grammatical gender in Russian. Journal of Child Language 39. 1077–1106. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000911000419

Rodina, Yulia & Westergaard, Marit. 2015. Grammatical gender in Norwegian: Language acquisition and language change. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 27(2). 145–187. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S1470542714000245

Rodina, Yulia & Westergaard, Marit. 2017. Grammatical gender in bilingual Norwegian-Russian acquisition: The role of input and transparency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20(1). 197–214. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000668

Ruberg, Tobias. 2013. Der Genuserwerb ein- und mehrsprachiger Kinder . Hamburg: Dr. Kovac.

Sabourin, Laura & Stowe, Laurie A. & de Haan, Ger J. 2006. Transfer effects in learning a second language grammatical gender system. Second Language Research 22(1). 1–29. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1191/0267658306sr259oa

Schiller, Niels O. & Caramazza, Alfonso. 2003. Grammatical feature selection in noun phrase production: Evidence from German and Dutch. Journal of Memory and Language 48(1). 169–194. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00508-9

Steinmetz, Donald. 2006. Gender shifts in Germanic and Slavic: semantic motivation for neuter? Lingua 116(9). 1418–1440. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2004.06.014

Stern, Clara & Stern, William. 1928. Die Kindersprache: Eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung . Leipzig: Barth.

Szagun, Gisela & Stumper, Barbara & Sondag, Nina & Franik, Melanie. 2007. The acquisition of gender marking by young German-speaking children: Evidence for learning guided by phonological regularities. Journal of Child Language 34(3). 445–471. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000906007951

Tracy, Rosemarie. 1986. The acquisition of case morphology in German. Linguistics 24. 47–78. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1986.24.1.47

Unsworth, Sharon & Argyri, Froso & Cornips, Leonie & Hulk, Aafke & Sorace, Antonella & Tsimpli, Ianthi. 2014. On the role of age of onset and input in early child bilingualism in Greek and Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics 35(4). 765–805. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000574

van Dijk, Chantal & vam Wonderen, Elise & Koutamanis, Elly & Kootstra, Gerrit Jan & Dijkstra, Ton & Unsworth, Sharon. 2021. Cross-linguistic influence in simultaneous and early sequential bilingual children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child Language . DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000921000337

Wegener, Heide. 1995. Das Genus im DaZ-Erwerb. Beobachtungen an Kindern aus Polen, Russland und in der Türkei. In Handwerker, Brigitte (ed.), Fremde Sprache Deutsch. Grammatische Beschreibung, Erwerbsverläufe, Lehrmethodik , 1–24. Tübingen: Narr.

Zubin, David & Köpcke, Klaus-Michael. 1986. Gender and folk taxonomy: The indexical relation between grammatical and lexical categorization. In Craig, Colette G. (ed.), Noun classes and categorization , 139–180. John Benjamins. DOI:   http://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.7.12zub

orcid logo

  • Download XML
  • Download PDF
  • Issue: 2022 • Volume 7

Supplementary Files

Creative Commons Attribution 4.0

Identifiers

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5696

Peer Review

This article has been peer reviewed.

File Checksums (MD5)

  • XML: 741cee0d23e67e3f9af491b3f7280bca
  • PDF: f09acc6143bc3cebf59df97ae9ee6545

Table of Contents

Non specialist summary.

This article has no summary

  • Skip to primary navigation
  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Skip to footer

Legal Templates

Home Business Assignment Agreement

Assignment Agreement Template

Use our assignment agreement to transfer contractual obligations.

Assignment Agreement Template

Updated February 1, 2024 Reviewed by Brooke Davis

An assignment agreement is a legal document that transfers rights, responsibilities, and benefits from one party (the “assignor”) to another (the “assignee”). You can use it to reassign debt, real estate, intellectual property, leases, insurance policies, and government contracts.

What Is an Assignment Agreement?

What to include in an assignment agreement, how to assign a contract, how to write an assignment agreement, assignment agreement sample.

trademark assignment agreement template

Partnership Interest

An assignment agreement effectively transfers the rights and obligations of a person or entity under an initial contract to another. The original party is the assignor, and the assignee takes on the contract’s duties and benefits.

It’s often a requirement to let the other party in the original deal know the contract is being transferred. It’s essential to create this form thoughtfully, as a poorly written assignment agreement may leave the assignor obligated to certain aspects of the deal.

The most common use of an assignment agreement occurs when the assignor no longer can or wants to continue with a contract. Instead of leaving the initial party or breaking the agreement, the assignor can transfer the contract to another individual or entity.

For example, imagine a small residential trash collection service plans to close its operations. Before it closes, the business brokers a deal to send its accounts to a curbside pickup company providing similar services. After notifying account holders, the latter company continues the service while receiving payment.

Create a thorough assignment agreement by including the following information:

  • Effective Date:  The document must indicate when the transfer of rights and obligations occurs.
  • Parties:  Include the full name and address of the assignor, assignee, and obligor (if required).
  • Assignment:  Provide details that identify the original contract being assigned.
  • Third-Party Approval: If the initial contract requires the approval of the obligor, note the date the approval was received.
  • Signatures:  Both parties must sign and date the printed assignment contract template once completed. If a notary is required, wait until you are in the presence of the official and present identification before signing. Failure to do so may result in having to redo the assignment contract.

Review the Contract Terms

Carefully review the terms of the existing contract. Some contracts may have specific provisions regarding assignment. Check for any restrictions or requirements related to assigning the contract.

Check for Anti-Assignment Clauses

Some contracts include anti-assignment clauses that prohibit or restrict the ability to assign the contract without the consent of the other party. If there’s such a clause, you may need the consent of the original parties to proceed.

Determine Assignability

Ensure that the contract is assignable. Some contracts, especially those involving personal services or unique skills, may not be assignable without the other party’s agreement.

Get Consent from the Other Party (if Required)

If the contract includes an anti-assignment clause or requires consent for assignment, seek written consent from the other party. This can often be done through a formal amendment to the contract.

Prepare an Assignment Agreement

Draft an assignment agreement that clearly outlines the transfer of rights and obligations from the assignor (the party assigning the contract) to the assignee (the party receiving the assignment). Include details such as the names of the parties, the effective date of the assignment, and the specific rights and obligations being transferred.

Include Original Contract Information

Attach a copy of the original contract or reference its key terms in the assignment agreement. This helps in clearly identifying the contract being assigned.

Execution of the Assignment Agreement

Both the assignor and assignee should sign the assignment agreement. Signatures should be notarized if required by the contract or local laws.

Notice to the Other Party

Provide notice of the assignment to the non-assigning party. This can be done formally through a letter or as specified in the contract.

File the Assignment

File the assignment agreement with the appropriate parties or entities as required. This may include filing with the original contracting party or relevant government authorities.

Communicate with Third Parties

Inform any relevant third parties, such as suppliers, customers, or service providers, about the assignment to ensure a smooth transition.

Keep Copies for Records

Keep copies of the assignment agreement, original contract, and any related communications for your records.

Here’s a list of steps on how to write an assignment agreement:

Step 1 – List the Assignor’s and Assignee’s Details

List all of the pertinent information regarding the parties involved in the transfer. This information includes their full names, addresses, phone numbers, and other relevant contact information.

This step clarifies who’s transferring the initial contract and who will take on its responsibilities.

Step 2 – Provide Original Contract Information

Describing and identifying the contract that is effectively being reassigned is essential. This step avoids any confusion after the transfer has been completed.

Step 3 – State the Consideration

Provide accurate information regarding the amount the assignee pays to assume the contract. This figure should include taxes and any relevant peripheral expenses. If the assignee will pay the consideration over a period, indicate the method and installments.

Step 4 – Provide Any Terms and Conditions

The terms and conditions of any agreement are crucial to a smooth transaction. You must cover issues such as dispute resolution, governing law, obligor approval, and any relevant clauses.

Step 5 – Obtain Signatures

Both parties must sign the agreement to ensure it is legally binding and that they have read and understood the contract. If a notary is required, wait to sign off in their presence.

Assignment Agreement Template

Related Documents

  • Purchase Agreement : Outlines the terms and conditions of an item sale.
  • Business Contract : An agreement in which each party agrees to an exchange, typically involving money, goods, or services.
  • Lease/Rental Agreement : A lease agreement is a written document that officially recognizes a legally binding relationship between two parties -- a landlord and a tenant.
  • Legal Resources
  • Partner With Us
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Policy
  • Do Not Sell My Personal Information

Assignment Agreement Template

The document above is a sample. Please note that the language you see here may change depending on your answers to the document questionnaire.

Thank you for downloading!

How would you rate your free template?

Click on a star to rate

German Parliament Votes to Make It Easier for Trans People to Change Their Legal Gender

T he German Parliament approved a law on Friday that will make it much easier for people to change their legal gender identities and first names.

The new law, which goes into effect on Nov. 1, allows trans people to change their legal gender by filling out a simple self-disclosure form to inform the registry offices. Under the previous law, trans people had to first be assessed by two psychiatrists and obtain permission from a court before they could legally change their gender. But now, under the self-determination act , name and gender changes will be processed within three months of filing the self-disclosure form. Individuals will also be able to change their legal gender identity to non-binary.

For children under the age of 14, the law will require parents to submit all relevant documentation, while children over the age of 14 would be able to submit the documentation themselves with the approval of their legal representatives. If the child’s legal representatives do not approve of the change, the child can still override their decision if they get approval from a family court. Once the person’s name and gender are changed, they cannot be changed again for at least one year.

The previous law in place dates back to 1980, and has been criticized by many trans activists as archaic and dehumanizing. German lawmaker, Nyke Slawik, who is transgender, said that under the current law, she spent over $2,000 obtaining assessments from doctors in order to change her legal gender. 

“We finally want to make it easier," said Slawik on ARD television. “Many other countries have gone this way, and Germany is simply following suit in significantly simplifying this registration.”

In 2023, Spain passed a law that allows individuals over the age of 16 to change their legal genders without any medical professionals’ involvement. Similarly in the U.K., the Scottish parliament passed a law that allows individuals to change their legal gender via a self-declaration so long as they are above the age of 16.

The bill has nevertheless come under attack by Germany’s right wing parties, and the German public remains divided on the issue. A poll from YouGov for the Welt am Sonntag newspaper showed that 46% of respondents were in favor of the bill while 41% against it.

More Must-Reads From TIME

  • Exclusive: Google Workers Revolt Over $1.2 Billion Contract With Israel
  • Jane Fonda Champions Climate Action for Every Generation
  • Stop Looking for Your Forever Home
  • The Sympathizer Counters 50 Years of Hollywood Vietnam War Narratives
  • The Bliss of Seeing the Eclipse From Cleveland
  • Hormonal Birth Control Doesn’t Deserve Its Bad Reputation
  • The Best TV Shows to Watch on Peacock
  • Want Weekly Recs on What to Watch, Read, and More? Sign Up for Worth Your Time

Contact us at [email protected]

  • Election 2024
  • Entertainment
  • Newsletters
  • Photography
  • Personal Finance
  • AP Investigations
  • AP Buyline Personal Finance
  • Press Releases
  • Israel-Hamas War
  • Russia-Ukraine War
  • Global elections
  • Asia Pacific
  • Latin America
  • Middle East
  • Election Results
  • Delegate Tracker
  • AP & Elections
  • March Madness
  • AP Top 25 Poll
  • Movie reviews
  • Book reviews
  • Personal finance
  • Financial Markets
  • Business Highlights
  • Financial wellness
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Social Media

Top UN court will hold hearings in a case accusing Germany of facilitating Israel’s Gaza conflict

Preliminary hearings open at the United Nations’ top court in a case that seeks an end to German military and other aid to Israel, based on claims that Berlin is ‘facilitating’ acts of genocide and breaches of international law in the Israel-Hamas war in Gaza.

Smoke rises following an Israeli airstrike in the Gaza Strip, as seen from southern Israel, Monday, Oct. 23, 2023. (AP Photo/Ariel Schalit)

Smoke rises following an Israeli airstrike in the Gaza Strip, as seen from southern Israel, Monday, Oct. 23, 2023. (AP Photo/Ariel Schalit)

  • Copy Link copied

Humanitarian aid is airdropped to Palestinians over Gaza City, Gaza Strip, Monday, March 25, 2024. (AP Photo/Mahmoud Essa)

Palestinians flee from northern Gaza as Israeli tanks block the Salah al-Din road in the central Gaza Strip on Friday, Nov. 24, 2023, as the four-day cease-fire in the Israel-Hamas war begins as part of an agreement that Qatar helped broker. (AP Photo/Mohammed Dahman)

THE HAGUE, Netherlands (AP) — Preliminary hearings open Monday at the United Nations’ top court in a case that seeks an end to German military and other aid to Israel, based on claims that Berlin is “facilitating” acts of genocide and breaches of international law in the Israel-Hamas war in Gaza.

Israel strongly denies its military campaign amounts to breaches of the Genocide Convention.

While the case brought by Nicaragua centers on Germany, it indirectly takes aim at Israel’s military campaign in Gaza following the deadly Oct. 7 attacks when Hamas-led militants stormed into southern Israel, killing some 1,200 people. More than 33,000 Palestinians have been killed in Gaza, according to the territory’s Health Ministry. Its toll doesn’t differentiate between civilians and combatants, but it has said women and children make up the majority of the dead.

“We are calm and we will set out our legal position in court,” German Foreign Ministry spokesperson Sebastian Fischer said ahead of the hearings.

“We reject Nicaragua’s accusations,” Fischer told reporters in Berlin on Friday. “Germany has breached neither the genocide convention nor international humanitarian law, and we will set this out in detail before the International Court of Justice.”

Israeli settlers gather near burning properties of Palestinian villagers after they set on fire, in the West Bank village of al-Mughayyir, Saturday, April 13, 2024. Israel's army says the body of a missing Israeli teen has been found in the occupied West Bank after he was killed in a "terrorist attack." The disappearance of 14-year-old Binyamin Achimair sparked a large attack by settlers on a Palestinian village on Friday and Saturday. (AP Photo/Nasser Nasser)

Nicaragua has asked the court to hand down preliminary orders known as provisional measures, including that Germany “immediately suspend its aid to Israel, in particular its military assistance including military equipment in so far as this aid may be used in the violation of the Genocide Convention” and international law.

The court will likely take weeks to deliver its preliminary decision and Nicaragua’s case will probably drag on for years.

Monday’s hearing at the world court comes amid growing calls for allies to stop supplying arms to Israel as its six-month campaign continues to lay waste to Gaza.

The offensive has displaced the vast majority of Gaza’s population. Food is scarce, the U.N. says famine is approaching and few Palestinians have been able to leave the besieged territory.

“The case next week in The Hague will likely further galvanize opposition to any support for Israel,” said Mary Ellen O’Connell, a professor of law and international peace studies at the University of Notre Dame.

On Friday, the U.N.’s top human rights body called on countries to stop selling or shipping weapons to Israel. The United States and Germany opposed the resolution.

Also, hundreds of British jurists, including three retired Supreme Court judges, have called on their government to suspend arms sales to Israel after three U.K. citizens were among seven aid workers from the charity World Central Kitchen killed in Israeli strikes . Israel said the attack on the aid workers was a mistake caused by “misidentification.”

Germany has for decades been a staunch supporter of Israel . Days after the Oct. 7 attack by Hamas, Chancellor Olaf Scholz explained why: “Our own history, our responsibility arising from the Holocaust, makes it a perpetual task for us to stand up for the security of the state of Israel,” he told lawmakers.

Berlin, however, has gradually shifted its tone as civilian casualties in Gaza have soared, becoming increasingly critical of the humanitarian situation in Gaza and spoken out against a ground offensive in Rafah.

Nicaragua’s government, which has historical links with Palestinian organizations dating back to their support for the 1979 Sandinista revolution, was itself accused earlier this year by U.N.-backed human rights experts of systematic human rights abuses “tantamount to crimes against humanity.” The government of President Daniel Ortega fiercely rejected the allegations.

In January, the ICJ imposed provisional measures ordering Israel to do all it can to prevent death, destruction and acts of genocide in Gaza. The orders came in a case filed by South Africa accusing Israel of breaching the Genocide Convention.

The court last week ordered Israel to take measures to improve the humanitarian situation in Gaza, including opening more land crossings to allow food, water, fuel and other supplies into the war-ravaged enclave.

On Friday, Israel said it’s taking steps to increase the flow of humanitarian aid into the Gaza Strip, including reopening a key border crossing into northern Gaza.

Nicaragua argues that by giving Israel political, financial and military support and by defunding the United Nations aid agency for Palestinians, UNRWA, “Germany is facilitating the commission of genocide and, in any case has failed in its obligation to do everything possible to prevent the commission of genocide.”

Israel strongly denies that its assault amounts to genocidal acts, saying it is acting in self defense. Israeli legal adviser Tal Becker told judges at the court in January that the country is fighting a “war it did not start and did not want.”

Associated Press writer Geir Moulson in Berlin contributed to this report.

assignment agreement in german

  • Look up in Linguee
  • Suggest as a translation of "assign"

Linguee Apps

▾ dictionary english-german, assign ( sth. ) verb ( assigned , assigned ) —, (etw. akk ) zuweisen v, übertragen v, beauftragen v, assign noun —, assign sth. to sb. verb ( assigned , assigned ) —, jdm. etw. akk zuordnen v, assign a claim v —, assign claims v —, assign duties v —, assign transfer v —, assign absolutely v —, assign a place v —, assign authorizations v —, assign property v —, assign by lot v —, assign proceeds v —, assign names v —, assign tasks to sb. v —, assign a patent to sb. v —, assign a day v —, assign the parts v —, ▾ external sources (not reviewed).

  • This is not a good example for the translation above.
  • The wrong words are highlighted.
  • It does not match my search.
  • It should not be summed up with the orange entries
  • The translation is wrong or of bad quality.
  • International edition
  • Australia edition
  • Europe edition

A woman looks at memorabilia and pictures of the hostages kidnapped in the 7 October attack, in Tel Aviv.

Hamas says it does not have 40 hostages who fit criteria for deal with Israel

US-backed proposal involves women, children and elderly or sick hostages in Gaza being exchanged for 900 Palestinian prisoners

  • Middle East crisis – live updates

The Palestinian militant group Hamas has indicated it does not have 40 captives who are still alive who meet the “humanitarian” criteria for a proposed hostages-for-prisoners ceasefire agreement with Israel .

A senior Israeli official confirmed claims made at the weekend by Hamas during talks in Cairo that it does not have 40 hostages in Gaza who meet the exchange criteria.

Ceasefire talks have focused on a US-backed proposal of a phased exchange of hostages and prisoners. In the first instance women, children, and elderly or sick people – including five female Israeli soldiers – would be exchanged for an estimated 900 Palestinian prisoners being held by Israel, alongside a six-week ceasefire in Gaza .

Hamas appears reluctant to make up the numbers for an exchange with surviving male hostages. Reliable information about how many hostages remain alive, who is holding them and where has been hard to come by.

The CIA director, William Burns, has presented a new proposal to try to bridge the gaps between the two sides.

The US is pressuring Israel to agree to release 900 Palestinian prisoners in the first phase of a three-stage deal as well as allowing the return of Palestinians to northern Gaza.

The talks, which resumed on Sunday, have brought no signs of a breakthrough on a plan presented by US, Qatari and Egyptian mediators, which Hamas said it was studying.

About 240 hostages, including the bodies of some killed during Hamas’s 7 October attack on southern Israel, were taken into Gaza during the assault.

So far 112 hostages have been returned alive to Israel. Of those, 105 were released as part of an exchange last year. Before that, Hamas released four prisoners unilaterally, while three more were rescued by the Israel Defense Forces.

In the months since 7 October, Israel has revealed that a number of those who were believed to have been alive when they were abducted were in fact killed during the initial Hamas attack.

Hamas has said some hostages have died during Israeli strikes on Gaza. In a high-profile friendly fire incident Israel killed three escaped male hostages as they approached Israeli troops.

Israel believes about 30 of the remaining hostages are dead, which would leave about 100 still alive, including 91 Israelis or dual nationals, eight Thai citizens, one Nepali, and one French-Mexican national.

The long-running manoeuvres on both sides around ceasefire negotiations – an increasingly politically contentious issue in Israel – have become more tortuous by the week despite pressure from mediators.

While there was speculation that Israel’s withdrawal of its forces from operations in southern Gaza may have been an undeclared confidence-building measure, the killing of three of the sons of Hamas’s political bureau chief, Ismail Haniyeh, along with several of his grandchildren, appeared to undercut that analysis.

In an interview with the Al Jazeera satellite channel, Haniyeh said the killings would not pressure Hamas into softening its positions.

Haniyeh left Gaza in 2019 and lives in exile in Qatar. The top Hamas leader in Gaza is Yahya Sinwar, who masterminded the 7 October attack on Israel, which killed about 1,200 people, mostly civilians.

Among the various stumbling blocks on the Israeli side are demands that displaced Palestinians be allowed to return to northern Gaza, as well as the identity of prisoners to be released from Israeli jails.

Hamas has been pushing for a far more significant cessation in hostilities, including a full withdrawal of Israeli forces from Gaza, even as Israeli officials have vowed to continue with the war.

Guardian Newsroom: Crisis in the Middle East On Tuesday 30 April, 7-8.15pm GMT, join Devika Bhat, Peter Beaumont, Emma Graham-Harrison and Ghaith Abdul-Ahad as they discuss the fast-developing crisis in the Middle East. Book tickets here or at theguardian.live

  • Israel-Gaza war
  • Palestinian territories
  • Middle East and north Africa

More on this story

assignment agreement in german

Iranian attack on Israel expected ‘sooner rather than later’, says Joe Biden

assignment agreement in german

Aid ‘still not reaching Gaza’, as top US official warns famine has started

assignment agreement in german

I believe in another Israel – one not defined by Benjamin Netanyahu and his cronies

assignment agreement in german

US seeking to deter Iran from strike on Israel, officials say

assignment agreement in german

Biden vows ‘ironclad’ US commitment to Israel amid fears of Iran attack

assignment agreement in german

Germany faces domestic lawsuit over its arms sales to Israel

assignment agreement in german

Israel’s security at core of German foreign policy due to Holocaust, ICJ hears

assignment agreement in german

‘A new abyss’: Gaza and the hundred years’ war on Palestine

assignment agreement in german

‘This cannot be Eid’: Gaza’s survivors mark the end of Ramadan in the shadow of war

Most viewed.

IMAGES

  1. Style and Formatting Agreement for German

    assignment agreement in german

  2. German Corporate Law: New Bilingual Resource

    assignment agreement in german

  3. Agreement germany (2)

    assignment agreement in german

  4. Assignment Agreement Template

    assignment agreement in german

  5. German (Assignment/Report) Template

    assignment agreement in german

  6. Contract Assignment Agreement

    assignment agreement in german

VIDEO

  1. German 1 Assignment 4 NPTL #nptel #german

  2. NPTEL German 1 Week 7 Assignment 7 Solved Question. #german #nptel #assignment7

  3. Hessians: The German Soldiers of the American Revolution #shorts #viral #short

  4. GERMAN GROUP ASSIGNMENT

  5. NPTEL German 1 Week 9 Assignment 9 Solved Question. #german #nptel #assignment8

  6. Get In Agreement With Your Assignment

COMMENTS

  1. assignment agreement

    RBC Dexia Investor Services Netherlands N.V. has assigned all its rights and obligations under the securities lending agency agreement to RBC Dexia Investor Services Trust (the "Lending agent") b y an Assignment Agreement date d August 29, 2008. gamax.de.

  2. assignment agreement

    Switch to mobile view. Learn the translation for 'assignment agreement' in LEO's ­English ⇔ German­ dictionary. With noun/verb tables for the different cases and tenses links to audio pronunciation and relevant forum discussions free vocabulary trainer .

  3. assignment agreement translation in German

    The assignment agreement stipulates all terms of the deal, including the principal and maturity date of the loan, as well as the interest rate.: Der Abtretungsvertrag enthält alle Bedingungen der Vereinbarung, einschließlich Kreditsumme und Fälligkeitsdatum des Kredits sowie Zinssatz.: A detailed description of the new structure is available in the Mintos Finance loan agreement and ...

  4. German Assignment Agreement

    German Assignment Agreement. This agreement is used when one party (the assignor) agrees to assign a contract, or the rights in a contract, or another income/object to a second party (the assignee). In exchange for the assignment, the assignee may give the assignor money or personal property, or forgive a debt or obligation. Simply answer the ...

  5. Assignment Agreement: What You Need to Know

    An assignment agreement is a contract that authorizes a person to transfer their rights, obligations, or interests in a contract or property to another person. It serves as a means for the assignor to delegate duties and advantages to a third party while the assignee assumes those privileges and obligations.

  6. German Law Assignment Agreement Definition

    Related to German Law Assignment Agreement. IP Assignment Agreement has the meaning set forth in Section 3.2(a)(iii). Lender Assignment Agreement means an assignment agreement substantially in the form of Exhibit D hereto. Patent Assignment Agreement means the patent assignment agreement substantially in the form of Exhibit A.

  7. assignment agreement employee

    Many translated example sentences containing "assignment agreement employee" - German-English dictionary and search engine for German translations.

  8. Commercial Contracts in Germany

    Yes. The principle of good faith (section 242, German Civil Code (BGB)) is a fundamental principle of law in Germany, which applies when contract negotiations commence. According to German law ...

  9. German translation of 'assignment'

    German Translation of "ASSIGNMENT" | The official Collins English-German Dictionary online. Over 100,000 German translations of English words and phrases.

  10. ASSIGNMENT

    1. general. assignment (also: assignments, conveyance, surrender, cession) volume_up. Abtretung {f} more_vert. A registration may be made in advance of the assignment to which it relates. expand_more Eine Eintragung kann vor der Abtretung, auf die sie sich bezieht, vorgenommen werden.

  11. PDF IP Assignment Clauses in German Employment Contracts

    It is important to note that German law does not share the concept of shop rights or hired-to-invent. However, there are provisions under German law that are not so different from these doctrines. The transfer of the rights in employee-made inventions to the employer is most peculiar in Germany. An advance assignment is not possible.

  12. assignment

    German assignment: Last post 27 Feb 21, 18:55: Under here is my German assignment (due tonight). I would really appreciate it if someone wo… 6 Replies: German assignment : Last post 26 Apr 21, 18:06: I would really appreciate it if someone would read through my German assignment, and let me … 8 Replies: assignment management: Last post 30 ...

  13. German Security Assignment Agreement

    IT IS AGREED as follows: . 1. DEFINITIONS AND INTERPRETATION. 1.1 Capitalised terms used in this German Security Assignment Agreement (the Agreement) shall, except where the context otherwise requires and save where otherwise defined in this Agreement, have the meanings given to them in the Master Definitions Agreement entered into between, inter alia, the Parties on 23 September 2009, and ...

  14. assignment

    assignment agreement: Zessionsvertrag {m} comp. assignment algorithm: Zuordnungsalgorithmus {m} assignment back: Rückübertragung {f} admin. insur. jobs assignment certificate: Entsendebescheinigung {f} law assignment clause: Zessionsklausel {f} law assignment contract: Abtretungsvertrag {m} assignment credit:

  15. agreement assignment

    Many translated example sentences containing "agreement assignment" - German-English dictionary and search engine for German translations.

  16. Intellectual Property Assignments from Software Developers: Key

    In assignment agreements between two legal entities, the assignee should ensure the assignor has first obtained a valid assignment of the rights from the human author. ... German software development agreements should contain licensing-type provisions. If properly drafted, the economic result of those provision will be same as a "full ...

  17. global assignment agreement

    sqs.de. sqs.de. Many translated example sentences containing "global assignment agreement" - German-English dictionary and search engine for German translations.

  18. ASSIGNMENT in German

    ASSIGNMENT translate: der Auftrag. Learn more in the Cambridge English-German Dictionary.

  19. Kupisch

    2.1 Gender assignment and agreement in German. German has a three-way gender system with masculine, feminine and neuter. Gender is marked on determiners (definite and indefinite articles, demonstratives, possessives), attributive adjectives and relative pronouns. Masculine is the most frequent and neuter the least frequent gender, although the ...

  20. Free Assignment Agreement Template

    Assignment Agreement Template. Use our assignment agreement to transfer contractual obligations. An assignment agreement is a legal document that transfers rights, responsibilities, and benefits from one party (the "assignor") to another (the "assignee"). You can use it to reassign debt, real estate, intellectual property, leases ...

  21. PDF Gender assignment and gender agreement in adult bilinguals and second

    German. Two experiments, an acceptability judgment task and an elicited pro-duction task, are carried out. Results show successful acquisition of agreement in all groups. By contrast, gender assignment may be mildly affected if French is acquired in a minority language context or as an L2. Keywords: gender, French, simultaneous bilingualism 1.

  22. Germany Makes Legal Gender Transition Easier

    April 12, 2024 10:20 AM EDT. T he German Parliament approved a law on Friday that will make it much easier for people to change their legal gender identities and first names. The new law, which ...

  23. Germany rejects allegations of genocidal acts in Gaza at UN court

    Tania von Uslar-Gleichen, Germany's legal adviser and Director-General for Legal Affairs of the German Foreign Ministry, center, shakes hands with Alain Pellet, left, a lawyer representing Nicaragua, prior to the start of a two days hearing at the World Court in The Hague, Netherlands, Monday, April 8, 2024, in a case brought by Nicaragua accusing Germany of breaching the genocide convention ...

  24. Germany faces UN hearing on accusations it facilitates Israel's Gaza

    THE HAGUE, Netherlands (AP) — Preliminary hearings open Monday at the United Nations' top court in a case that seeks an end to German military and other aid to Israel, based on claims that Berlin is "facilitating" acts of genocide and breaches of international law in the Israel-Hamas war in Gaza.. Israel strongly denies its military campaign amounts to breaches of the Genocide Convention.

  25. assign

    A general partner shall have all the rights and powers and be subject to all the restrictions and liabilities of a partner in a partnership without limited partners, except that without the written consent or ratification of the specific act by all the limited partners, a general partner or all the general partners have no authority to do any one or more of the following: do any act in ...

  26. assignment agreements

    Learn the translation for 'assignment\x20agreements' in LEO's ­English ⇔ German­ dictionary. With noun/verb tables for the different cases and tenses links to audio pronunciation and relevant forum discussions free vocabulary trainer

  27. German airports reach agreement to end strike action

    German airports reach agreement to end strike action 04/08/2024 April 8, 2024. Airport operators in Germany have agreed on a deal with security staff after a series of strikes at some of the ...

  28. Hamas says it does not have 40 hostages who fit criteria for deal with

    The Palestinian militant group Hamas has indicated it does not have 40 captives who are still alive who meet the "humanitarian" criteria for a proposed hostages-for-prisoners ceasefire ...