Book cover

The Crime Analyst's Companion pp 193–211 Cite as

Problem-Solving and SARA

  • Iain Agar 4  
  • First Online: 28 April 2022

558 Accesses

4 Altmetric

Criminologist Herman Goldstein articulated the problem-oriented approach to policing (hereafter, POP) in 1979, recognising that many of the isolated incidents responded to by police are symptomatic of more substantive problems rooted within a disparate array of social and environmental conditions. The basic elements of problem-solving, and indeed problem-solving analysis, begin with grouping incidents as problems and putting them at the heart of policing – the problem becoming a unit of police work (Goldstein H, Problem oriented policing. Temple Univ. Pr, Philadelphia, 1990). The aim of problem-solving is to improve policing by enabling the most efficient use of our finite resources to serve the public effectively. In this chapter, we will work through the organisational theory of POP from the analyst perspective, broken down into two parts – the active role of the analyst in problem-solving and identifying suitable responses, followed by an extended breakdown of the stages in a SARA model and how you can become a problem-solving crime analyst.

  • Problem-oriented policing
  • Problem-solving analysis

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution .

Buying options

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

https://popcenter.asu.edu/pop-guides

What Works Crime Reduction Toolkit https://whatworks.college.police.uk/toolkit/Pages/Welcome.aspx

Evidence-Based Policing Matrix https://cebcp.org/evidence-based-policing/the-matrix/

Crime Science Journal https://crimesciencejournal.biomedcentral.com/

Cambridge Journal of Evidence-Based Policing https://www.springer.com/journal/41887

Allison, P., & Allison, P. D. (2001). Missing data . University of Pennsylvania.

Google Scholar  

Ariel, B., Bland, M., & Sutherland, A. (2021). Experimental designs . Sage.

Ashby, M. P. J. (2020). Initial evidence on the relationship between the coronavirus pandemic and crime in the United States. Crime Science, 9 (1). Available at: https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/ep87s/ . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Bachman, R., & Schutt, R. K. (2020). Fundamentals of research in criminology and criminal justice: Fifth Edition . Sage Publications, Inc.

Birks, D., Coleman, A., & Jackson, D. (2020). Unsupervised identification of crime problems from police free-text data. Crime Science, 9 (1). Available at: https://crimesciencejournal.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s40163-020-00127-4 . Accessed 25 Feb 2021.

Boba, R. (2003). Problem analysis in policing . Police Foundation. Available at: https://popcenter.asu.edu/sites/default/files/library/reading/pdfs/problemanalysisinpolicing.pdf . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Boba Santos, R. (2017). Crime analysis with crime mapping . Sage Publications, Inc.

Borrion, H., Ekblom, P., Alrajeh, D., Borrion, A. L., Keane, A., Koch, D., Mitchener-Nissen, T., & Toubaline, S. (2020). The problem with crime problem-solving: Towards a second generation pop? The British Journal of Criminology, 60 (1).

Bowers, K., Tompson, L., Sidebottom, A., Bullock, K., & Johnson, S. D. (2017). Reviewing evidence for evidence-based policing. In J. Knutsson & L. Tompson (Eds.), Advances in evidence-based policing . Routledge.

Brantingham, P., & Brantingham, P. (1995). Criminality of place. European Journal on Criminal Policy and Research, 3 (3), 5–26.

Article   Google Scholar  

Brayley, H., Cockbain, E., & Laycock, G. (2011). The value of crime scripting: Deconstructing internal child sex trafficking. Policing, 5 (2), 132–143. Available at: https://academic.oup.com/policing/article/5/2/132/1518450 . Accessed 25 Feb 2021.

Brown, R., Scott, M. S., & Remington, F. J. (2007). Implementing responses to problems . United States of America 07. Available at: https://popcenter.asu.edu/sites/default/files/implementing_responses_to_problems.pdf . Accessed 25 Feb 2021.

Burton, S., & McGregor, M. (2018). Enhancing SARA: A new approach in an increasingly complex world. Crime Science, 7 (1).

Chainey, S. (2012). Improving the explanatory content of analysis products using hypothesis testing. Policing, 6 (2), 108–121.

Clarke, R. V., & Eck, J. (2003). Becoming a problem-solving crime analyst in 55 small steps . Jill Dando Institute of Crime Science, University College London. Available at: https://popcenter.asu.edu/sites/default/files/library/reading/PDFs/55stepsUK.pdf . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Clarke, R. V. G., & Schultze, P. A. (2005). Researching a problem . U.S. Dept. Of Justice, Office Of Community Oriented Policing Services. Available at: https://popcenter.asu.edu/sites/default/files/researching_a_problem.pdf . Accessed 25 Feb 2021.

Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. (1979). Social change and crime rate trends: A routine activity approach. American Sociological Review , [online] 44 (4), 588–608.

Cornish, D. B., & Clarke, R. V. (1986). The reasoning criminal: Rational choice perspectives on offending . Transaction Publishers.

Book   Google Scholar  

Cornish, D. B., & Clarke, R. V. (2016). The rational choice perspective. In R. Wortley & M. Townsley (Eds.), Environmental criminology and crime analysis (2nd ed.). Routledge.

Eck, J. E., & Spelman, W. (1987). Problem-solving: Problem-oriented policing in Newport News . U.S. Dept. Of Justice, National Institute Of Justice. Available at: https://www.ojp.gov/pdffiles1/Digitization/111964NCJRS.pdf . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Eck, J. E. (2017). Assessing responses to problems: an introductory guide for police problem-solvers: 2nd Edition . U.S. Dept. Of Justice, Office Of Community Oriented Policing Services. Available at: https://popcenter.asu.edu/sites/default/files/assessing_responses_to_problems_final.pdf . Accessed 07 Mar 2021.

Eck, J.E. (2019). Advocate: Why Problem-Oriented Policing. In: D. Weisburd and A.A. Braga, eds., Police Innovation: Contrasting Perspectives. United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

Ekblom, P. (2005). The 5Is framework: Sharing good practice in crime prevention. In E. Marks, A. Meyer, & R. Linssen (Eds.), Quality in crime prevention . Hannover.

Ekblom, P., & Pease, K. (1995). Evaluating crime prevention. In M. Tonry & D. Farrington (Eds.), From building a safer society: Strategic approaches to crime prevention (Vol. 19, pp 585–662). United States.

Goldstein, H. (1990). Problem oriented policing . Temple Univ. Pr.

Goldstein, H. (2003). Problem analysis in policing . Police Foundation.

Guilfoyle, S. (2013). Intelligent policing: how systems thinking methods eclipse conventional management practice . Triarchy Press.

Heeks, M., Reed, S., Tafsiri, M., & Prince, S. (2018). Economic and social costs of crime . Home Office. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/732110/the-economic-and-social-costs-of-crime-horr99.pdf Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Heuer, R. J. (1999). Psychology of intelligence analysis. Washington, D.C.: Center For The Study Of Intelligence, Central Intelligence Agency.

Hinkle, J. C., Weisburd, D., Telep, C. W., & Petersen, K. (2020). Problem-oriented policing for reducing crime and disorder: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Campbell Systematic Reviews, 16 (2).

Johnson, S. D., Tilley, N., & Bowers, K. J. (2015). Introducing EMMIE: An evidence rating scale to encourage mixed-method crime prevention synthesis reviews. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 11 (3), 459–473.

Kennedy, D. M. (2020). Problem-oriented public safety. In M. S. Scott & R. V. Clarke (Eds.), Problem-oriented policing successful case studies . Routledge.

Kime, S., & Wheller, L. (2018). The policing evaluation toolkit . College of Policing. Available at: https://whatworks.college.police.uk/Support/Documents/The_Policing_Evaluation_Toolkit.pdf . Accessed 7 Mar 2021.

Kuang, D., Brantingham, P. J., & Bertozzi, A. L. (2017). Crime topic modeling. Crime Science, 6 (1). Available at: https://crimesciencejournal.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s40163-017-0074-0 . Accessed 25 Feb 2021.

Ratcliffe, J. (2008). Intelligence-led policing . Willan Publishing.

Ratcliffe, J. H. (2014). Towards an index for harm-focused policing. Policing, 9 (2), 164–182. Available at: https://academic.oup.com/policing/article/9/2/164/1451352 . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Ratcliffe, J. (2016). Intelligence-led policing (2nd ed.). Routledge.

Ratcliffe, J. (2018). Reducing crime: A companion for police leaders . Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.

Scott, M., & Kirby, S. (2012). Implementing POP: Leading, structuring and managing a Problem-Oriented Police Agency . Community Oriented Policing Services, U.S. Department of Justice, Washington, DC. Available at: https://eprints.lancs.ac.uk/id/eprint/60830/1/popmanualfinal_copy.pdf . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Sherman, L. W. (1997). Preventing crime: what works, what doesn’t, what’s promising: A report to the United States Congress . College Park, Md Univ. Of Maryland. Available at: https://www.ojp.gov/pdffiles/171676.pdf . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Sherman, L.W. (2020). How to count crime: The Cambridge harm index consensus. Cambridge Journal of Evidence-Based Policing .

Sidebottom, A., & Tilley, N. (2011). Improving problem-oriented policing: The need for a new model? Crime Prevention and Community Safety, 13 (2), 79–101.

Sidebottom, A., Tilley, N., & Eck, J. E. (2012). Towards checklists to reduce common sources of problem-solving failure. Policing, 6 (2), 194–209.

Sidebottom, A., Bullock, K., Ashby, M., Kirby, S., Armitage, R., Laycock, G., & Tilley, N. (2020). Successful police problem-solving: A practice guide . Jill Dando Institute of Security and Crime Science, University College London. Available at: https://popcenter.asu.edu/sites/default/files/successful_police_problem_solving_a_guide.pdf . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Skogan, W. G. (1977). Dimensions of the dark figure of unreported crime. Crime & Delinquency, 23 (1), 41–50.

Telep, C. W., & Weisburd, D. (2012). What is known about the effectiveness of police practices in reducing crime and disorder? Police Quarterly, 15 (4), 331–357.

Tilley, N., Read, T. and Great Britain. Home Office. Research, Development And Statistics Directorate. Policing And Reducing Crime Unit. (2000). Not rocket science?: problem-solving and crime reduction: Paper 6 . Home Office, Policing And Reducing Crime Unit. Available at: https://popcenter.asu.edu/sites/default/files/library/reading/pdfs/Rocket_Science.pdf . Accessed 25 Feb 2021.

Tilley, N. (2008). Crime Prevention . Willan Pub..

Townsley, M., Mann, M., & Garrett, K. (2011). The missing link of crime analysis: A systematic approach to testing competing hypotheses. Policing, 5 (2), 158–171. Available at: https://eprints.qut.edu.au/91925/ . Accessed 25 Feb 2021.

Walker, J. T., & Drawve, G. R. (2018). Foundations of crime analysis: Data, analyses, and mapping . Routledge.

Weisburd, D., Telep, C. W., Hinkle, J. C., & Eck, J. E. (2010). Is problem-oriented policing effective in reducing crime and disorder? Criminology & Public Policy, 9 (1), 139–172.

Weisburd, D., Hinkle, J. C., & Telep, C. (2019). Updated protocol: The effects of problem-oriented policing on crime and disorder: An updated systematic review. Campbell Systematic Reviews, 15 (1–2). Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/cl2.1089 . Accessed 15 Jan 2021.

Wheeler, A. P. (2016). Tables and graphs for monitoring temporal crime trends. International Journal of Police Science & Management, 18 (3), 159–172.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Essex Police, Chelmsford, UK

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iain Agar .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Institute of Criminology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Matthew Bland

Barak Ariel

Norfolk and Suffolk Constabularies, Ipswich, UK

Natalie Ridgeon

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter.

Agar, I. (2022). Problem-Solving and SARA. In: Bland, M., Ariel, B., Ridgeon, N. (eds) The Crime Analyst's Companion. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-94364-6_14

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-94364-6_14

Published : 28 April 2022

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-94363-9

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-94364-6

eBook Packages : Law and Criminology Law and Criminology (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research
  • Short Contribution
  • Open access
  • Published: 01 March 2018

Enhancing SARA: a new approach in an increasingly complex world

  • Steve Burton 1 &
  • Mandy McGregor 2  

Crime Science volume  7 , Article number:  4 ( 2018 ) Cite this article

37k Accesses

2 Citations

11 Altmetric

Metrics details

The research note describes how an enhancement to the SARA (Scan, Analyse, Respond and Assess) problem-solving methodology has been developed by Transport for London for use in dealing with crime and antisocial behaviour, road danger reduction and reliability problems on the transport system in the Capital. The revised methodology highlights the importance of prioritisation, effective allocation of intervention resources and more systematic learning from evaluation.

Introduction

Problem oriented policing (POP), commonly referred to as problem-solving in the UK, was first described by Goldstein ( 1979 , 1990 ) and operationalised by Eck and Spelman ( 1987 ) using the SARA model. SARA is the acronym for Scanning, Analysis, Response and Assessment. It is essentially a rational method to systematically identify and analyse problems, develop specific responses to individual problems and subsequently assess whether the response has been successful (Weisburd et al. 2008 ).

A number of police agencies around the world use this approach, although its implementation has been patchy, has often not been sustained and is particularly vulnerable to changes in the commitment of senior staff and lack of organisational support (Scott and Kirby 2012 ). This short contribution outlines the way in which SARA has been used and further developed by Transport for London (TfL, the strategic transport authority for London) and its policing partners—the Metropolitan Police Service, British Transport Police and City of London Police. Led by TfL, they have been using POP techniques to deal with crime and disorder issues on the network, with some success. TfL’s problem-solving projects have been shortlisted on three occasions for the Goldstein Award, an international award that recognises excellence in POP initiatives, winning twice in 2006 and 2011 (see Goldstein Award Winners 1993–2010 ).

Crime levels on the transport system are derived from a regular and consistent data extract from the Metropolitan Police Service and British Transport Police crime recording systems. In 2006, crime levels on the bus network were causing concern. This was largely driven by a sudden rise in youth crime with a 72 per cent increase from 2005 to 2006: The level rose from around 290 crimes involving one or more suspects aged under 16 years per month in 2005 to around 500 crimes per month in 2006.

Fear of crime was also an issue and there were increasing public and political demands for action. In response TfL, with its policing partners, worked to embed a more structured and systematic approach to problem-solving, allowing them to better identify, manage and evaluate their activities. Since then crime has more than halved on the network (almost 30,000 fewer crimes each year) despite significant increases in passenger journeys (Fig.  1 ). This made a significant contribution to the reduction in crime from 20 crimes per million passenger journeys in 2005/6 to 7.5 in 2016/17.

Crime volumes and rates on major TfL transport networks and passenger levels

Although crime has being falling generally over the last decade, the reduction on London’s public transport network has been comparatively greater than that seen overall in London and in England and Wales (indexed figures can be seen in Fig.  2 ). The reductions on public transport are even more impressive given that there are very few transport-related burglary and vehicle crimes which have been primary drivers of the overall reductions seen in London and England and Wales. TfL attributes this success largely to its problem-solving approach and the implementation of a problem-solving framework and supporting processes.

UK, London and transport crime trends since 2005/6

A need for change

TfL remains fully committed to problem-solving and processes are embedded within its transport policing, enforcement and compliance activities. However, it has become apparent that its approach needs to develop further in response to a number of emerging issues:

broadening of SARA beyond a predominant crime focus to address road danger reduction and road reliability problems;

increasing strategic complexity in the community safety and policing arena for example, the increased focus on safeguarding and vulnerability;

the increasing pace of both social and technological change, for example, sexual crimes such as ‘upskirting’ and ‘airdropping of indecent images’ (see http://www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/crime/london-tube-sexual-assault-underground-transportation-harassment-a8080756.html );

financial challenges and resource constraints yet growing demands for policing and enforcement action to deal with issues;

greater focus on a range of non-enforcement interventions as part of problem solving responses;

a small upturn in some crime types including passenger aggression and low-level violence when the network is at peak capacity;

increasing focus on evidence-led policing and enforcement, and;

some evidence of cultural fatigue among practitioners with processes which indicated a refresh of the approach might be timely.

Implications

In response, TfL undertook a review of how SARA and its problem-solving activities are being delivered and considered academic reviews and alternative models such as the 5I’s as developed by Ekblom ( 2002 ) and those assessed by Sidebottom and Tilley ( 2011 ). This review resulted in a decision to continue with a SARA-style approach because of its alignment with existing processes and the practitioner base that had already been established around SARA. This has led to a refresh of TfL’s strategic approach to managing problem-solving which builds on SARA and aims to highlight the importance of prioritisation, effective allocation of intervention resources and capturing the learning from problem-solving activities at a strategic and tactical level. Whilst these stages are implicit within the SARA approach, it was felt that a more explicit recognition of their importance as component parts of the process would enhance overall problem-solving efforts undertaken by TfL and its policing partners. The revised approach, which recognises these important additional steps in the problem-solving process, has been given the acronym SPATIAL—Scan, Prioritise, Analyse, Task, Intervene, Assess and Learn as defined in Table  1 below:

SPATIAL adapts the SARA approach to address a number of emerging common issues affecting policing and enforcement agencies over recent years. The financial challenges now facing many organisations mean that limited budgets and constrained resources are inadequate to be able to solve all problems identified. The additional steps in the SPATIAL process help to ensure that there is (a) proper consideration and prioritisation of identified ‘problems’ (b) effective identification and allocation of resources to deal with the problem, considering the impact on other priorities and (c) capture of learning from the assessment of problem-solving efforts so that evidence of what works (including an assessment of process, cost, implementation and impact) can be incorporated in the development of problem-solving action and response plans where appropriate. The relationship between SARA and SPATIAL is shown in Fig.  3 below:

SARA and SPATIAL

In overall terms SPATIAL helps to ensure that TfL and policing partners’ problem-solving activities are developed, coordinated and managed in a more structured way. Within TfL problem-solving is implemented at three broad levels—Strategic, Tactical and Operational. Where problems and activities sit within these broad levels depends on the timescale, geographic spread, level of harm and profile. These can change over time. Operational activities continue to be driven by a problem-solving process based primarily on SARA as they do not demand the same level of resource prioritisation and scale of evaluation, with a SPATIAL approach applied at a strategic level. In reality a number of tactical/operational problem-oriented policing activities will form a subsidiary part of strategic problem-solving plans. Table  2 provides examples of problems at these three levels.

The processes supporting delivery utilise existing well established practices used by TfL and its partners. These include Transtat (the joint TfL/MPS version of the ‘CompStat’ performance management process for transport policing), a strategic tasking meeting (where the ‘P’ in SPATIAL is particularly explored) and an Operations Hub which provides deployment oversight and command and control services for TfL’s on-street resources. Of course, in reality these processes are not always sequential. In many cases there will be feedback loops to allow refocusing of the problem definition and re-assessment of problem-solving plans and interventions.

For strategic and tactical level problems, the SPATIAL framework provides senior officers with greater oversight of problem-solving activity at all stages of the problem-solving process. It helps to ensure that TfL and transport policing resources are focussed on the right priorities, that the resource allocation is appropriate across identified priorities and that there is oversight of the problem-solving approaches being adopted, progress against plans and delivery of agreed outcomes.

Although these changes are in the early stages of implementation, it is already clear that they provide the much needed focus around areas such as strategic prioritisation and allocation of TfL, police and other partner resources (including officers and other interventions such as marketing, communications and environmental changes). The new approach also helps to ensure that any lessons learned from the assessment are captured and used to inform evidence-based interventions for similar problems through the use of a bespoke evaluation framework (adaptation of the Maryland scientific methods scale, see Sherman et al. 1998 ) and the implementation of an intranet based library. The adapted approach also resonates with practitioners because it builds on the well-established SARA process but brings additional focus to prioritising issues and optimising resources. More work is required to assess the medium and longer term implications and benefits derived from the new process and this will be undertaken as it becomes more mature.

Eck, J., & Spelman, W. (1987). Problem - solving: problem - oriented policing in newport news . Washington, D.C.: Police Executive Research Forum. https://www.ncjrs.gov/App/Publications/abstract.aspx?ID=111964 .

Ekblom, P. (2002). From the source to the mainstream is uphill: The challenge of transferring knowledge of crime prevention through replication, innovation 5 design against crime paper the 5Is framework and anticipation. In N. Tilley (Ed.), Analysis for crime prevention, crime prevention studies (Vol. 13, pp. 131–203).

Goldstein, H. (1979). Improving Policing: A Problem-Oriented Approach. Univ. of Wisconsin Legal Studies Research Paper No. 1336. pp. 236–258. SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2537955 . Accessed 21 Jan 2018.

Goldstein, H. (1990). Problem-oriented policing create space independent publishing platform. ISBN-10: 1514809486.

Goldstein Award Winners (1993–2010), Center for Problem-Oriented Policing. http://www.popcenter.org/library/awards/goldstein/ . Accessed 21 Jan 2018.

Scott, M. S., & Kirby, S. (2012). Implementing POP: leading, structuring and managing a problem-oriented police agency . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice Office of Community Oriented Policing Services.

Google Scholar  

Sidebottom, A., & Tilley, N. (2011). Improving problem-oriented policing: the need for a new model. Crime Prevention and Community Safety, 13 (2), 79–101.

Article   Google Scholar  

Sherman, L., Gottfredson, D., MacKenzie D., Eck, J., Reuter, P. & Bushway, S. (1988). Preventing Crime: What Works, What Doesn’t, What’s Promising. National Institute of Justice Research Brief. https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles/171676.pdf . Accessed 14 Dec 2017.

Weisburd, D., Telep, C.W., Hinkle, J. C., & Eck, J. E. (2008). The effects of problem oriented policing on crime and disorder. https://www.campbellcollaboration.org/media/k2/attachments/1045_R.pdf . Accessed 21 Jan 2018.

Download references

Authors’ contributions

The article was co-authored by the two named authors. SB developed the original concept and developed the methodology and MM helped refine the ideas for practical implementation and provided additional content to the document. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Ethical approval and consent to participate, competing interests.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

60 Fordwich Rise, London, SG14 2BN, UK

Steve Burton

Transport for London, London, UK

Mandy McGregor

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steve Burton .

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Burton, S., McGregor, M. Enhancing SARA: a new approach in an increasingly complex world. Crime Sci 7 , 4 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40163-018-0078-4

Download citation

Received : 18 September 2017

Accepted : 19 February 2018

Published : 01 March 2018

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s40163-018-0078-4

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Problem solving
  • Transport policing
  • Problem oriented policing

Crime Science

ISSN: 2193-7680

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Campbell Syst Rev
  • v.16(2); 2020 Jun

Logo of csysrev

Problem‐oriented policing for reducing crime and disorder: An updated systematic review and meta‐analysis

Joshua c. hinkle.

1 Department of Criminal Justice and Criminology, Georgia State University, Atlanta Georgia

David Weisburd

2 Criminology, Law and Society, George Mason University, Fairfax Virginia

3 Institute of Criminology, Faculty of Law, Hebrew University, Jerusalem Israel

Cody W. Telep

4 School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University, Phoenix Arizona

Kevin Petersen

Herman Goldstein developed problem‐oriented policing (POP) to focus police on more proactively addressing chronic problems, rather than using traditional reactive efforts. POP has been utilized to target a wide range of problems and has become commonly used in agencies across the United States and the world, although implementation is often uneven. POP interventions commonly use the SARA (scanning, analysis, response, assessment) model to identify problems, carefully analyze the conditions contributing to the problem, develop a tailored response to target these underlying factors, and evaluate outcome effectiveness.

To extend and update the findings of the original POP systematic review by synthesizing the findings of published and unpublished evaluations of POP through December 2018 to assess its overall impacts on crime and disorder. The review also examined impacts of POP on crime displacement, police financial costs, and noncrime outcomes.

Search Methods

Searches using POP keywords of the Global Policing Database at the University of Queensland were conducted to identify published and unpublished evaluations between 2006 and 2018. We supplemented these searches with forward searches, hand searches of leading journals and the Center for Problem‐Oriented Policing, and consultation with experts.

Selection Criteria

Eligible studies had to include a target area or group that received a POP intervention AND a control area/group that received standard police services. The control condition could be either experimental or quasi‐experimental. Units of analysis could be places or people. We defined POP as studies that generally followed the tenets of the SARA model.

Data Collection and Analysis

We identified 39 new (published between 2006 and 2018) studies that met our eligibility criteria as an evaluation of POP. Twenty‐four of these studies had sufficient data available to calculate an effect size. Along with the 10 studies from our initial systematic review of POP, these 34 studies are included in our meta‐analytic review of POP. Nine of these studies were randomized experiments and 25 were quasi‐experiments. We calculated effect sizes for each study using Cohen's D and relative incidence risk ratios and used random effects meta‐analyses to synthesize studies.

Our meta‐analyses suggest statistically significant impacts of POP. Our relative incident risk ratio analysis of mean effects suggests a 33.8% reduction in crime/disorder in the POP treatment areas/groups relative to the controls. We find no evidence of significant crime displacement as a result of POP and some evidence for a greater likelihood of a diffusion of crime control benefits. Few studies assessed noncrime outcomes, but our narrative review suggests POP is cost‐effective, but has limited impacts on fear of crime, legitimacy, and collective efficacy.

Authors’ Conclusions

Our review provides strong and consistent evidence that POP is an effective strategy for reducing crime and disorder. There is a great deal of heterogeneity in the magnitude of effect sizes across factors such as study type, study rigor and crime type. Despite this heterogeneity, 31 out of 34 studies (91.2%) have effect sizes in favor of a treatment effect and the overall mean effect is positive and significant in all of our models.

1. PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY

1.1. problem‐oriented policing (pop) is associated with reductions in crime and disorder.

POP is associated with statistically significant reductions in crime and disorder. Place‐based POP programs are more likely to produce a diffusion of benefits into areas adjacent to targeted locations than to lead to crime displacement.

1.2. What is this review about?

POP is a proactive policing strategy developed by Herman Goldstein, who argued that the standard reactive model of policing was ineffective as it was overly focused on the means of policing (number of arrests, average response time, etc.) rather than the end goal of reducing crime and enhancing community safety. He suggested that police could be more effective if they were more proactive and researched root causes of crime, and developed tailor‐made responses.

This review assesses the effectiveness of POP interventions—defined as those programs which generally followed the tenets of the SARA model (scanning, analysis, response, assessment) developed by Spelman and Eck—in reducing crime and disorder and fear of crime, and improving citizen perceptions of police.

What is the aim of this review?

This update of a Campbell systematic review assesses the effectiveness of problem‐oriented policing in reducing crime and disorder. It summarises the evidence from 34 studies: 28 from the United States, five from the United Kingdom, and one from Canada.

1.3. What studies are included?

This review includes both randomized and quasi‐experimental evaluations of POP, where a treatment area or group received a POP approach while a control area or group received standard police services.

Thirty‐four studies are assessed in the review—an increase of 24 studies from the original review (Weisburd, Telep, Hinkle, & Eck,  2008 ; Weisburd, Telep, Hinkle, & Eck,  2010 ). All studies were published between 1989 and 2018. Most studies (28) were conducted in the United States, five in the United Kingdom, and one in Canada.

1.4. Does POP reduce crime and disorder?

Yes. The results of this updated systematic review suggest that POP is associated with a statistically significant overall reduction in crime and disorder of 34%.

There are positive impacts for POP across a wide variety of crime and disorder outcomes, among studies that targeted problem places and problem people, at a variety of different units of analysis and featuring a wide array of types of interventions. The effect size is smaller in randomized experiments and after accounting for publication bias.

POP had limited impacts on police legitimacy, fear of crime, and collective efficacy. Few studies incorporated cost‐benefit analyses, but those that did suggest POP can be cost‐effective and provide substantial savings through prevented calls‐for‐service and incidents.

1.5. What do the findings of the review mean?

Findings from this review support the notion that proactive policing strategies that identify specific problems, conduct analyses to determine underlying causes, and develop and deliver tailor‐made responses, are more effective in reducing crime and disorder than standard, reactive methods of policing. Moreover, in place‐based interventions, diffusion of crime‐reduction benefits are more likely than displacing crime to nearby areas. As such, police departments should incorporate the use of problem‐solving into their crime prevention strategies.

However, the impacts of POP on crime are highly heterogeneous. This result may reflect the tremendous variability in the types of problems identified and targeted and the types of tailored intervention strategies used, suggesting that more studies are needed to allow more robust analyses of factors that influence POP program impacts. In turn, future evaluations should be designed to capture more data about the problem‐solving process so that future reviews can more directly assess what types of problems seem most amenable to POP efforts and what characteristics of problem‐solving interventions are associated with larger effects.

1.6. How up‐to‐date is this review?

This authors of this review update searched for studies up to December 2018.

2. BACKGROUND

2.1. the issue.

In an article in Crime & Delinquency in 1979, Herman Goldstein critiqued police practices of the time by noting that they were more focused on the “means” of policing than the “ends” or goals of policing. His critique drew from a series of recently completed studies that suggested that such standard policing practices as “preventive patrol” (Kelling, Pate, Dieckman, & Brown,  1974 ) or “rapid patrol car response to calls for service” (Kansas City Police Department,  1977 ) had little impact on crime. Goldstein suggested that the research evidence was not idiosyncratic but reflected a crisis in policing. To illustrate his concern, he referred to a newspaper article in the United Kingdom that reported on bus drivers in a small city that were passing bus stops waving and smiling but failing to pick up passengers. When questioned by a reporter, a representative for the bus company responded that “it is impossible for the drivers to keep their timetable if they have to stop for passengers” (Goldstein,  1979 , p. 236). Goldstein termed this the “means over ends syndrome” and noted that police were particularly susceptible to this problem. Goldstein noted that the police too had become so focused on the means of policing—such issues as the staffing and management of police—that they had begun to ignore the problems policing was meant to solve. Goldstein saw this dysfunction as at the heart of the failures of the police to be effective in addressing community problems.

Goldstein called for a paradigm shift in policing that would replace the primarily reactive, incident driven “standard model of policing” (Skogan & Frydl,  2004 ; Weisburd & Eck,  2004 ) with a model that required the police to be proactive in identifying underlying problems that could be targeted to alleviate crime and disorder at their roots. He termed this new approach “problem‐oriented policing” to accentuate its call for police to focus on problems and not on the everyday management of police agencies. Goldstein also expanded the traditional mandate of policing beyond crime and law enforcement. He argued that the police should deal with an array of problems in the community, including not only crime, but also social and physical disorder.

He also called for police to expand the tools of policing much beyond the law enforcement powers that are seen as the predominant tools of the standard model of policing. In Goldstein's view, the police needed to draw upon not only the criminal law, but also civil statutes and rely on other municipal and community resources if they were to successfully ameliorate crime and disorder problems. As such, successful implementations of POP would be reliant on forming partnerships with other agencies, community organizations and community members to deliver non‐law enforcement responses. This would particularly be the case when the targeted problems do not necessarily involve law violations.

John Eck and William Spelman (1987) drew upon Goldstein's idea to create a straightforward model for implementing POP, which has become widely accepted. In an application of problem solving in Newport News, in which Goldstein acted as a consultant, they developed the SARA model for problem solving. SARA is an acronym representing four steps they suggest police should follow when implementing POP, which will be outlined in Section  2.2 . 1

A 2004 report from the National Research Council offered the following description of POP and how the SARA model works in practice:

The heart of problem‐oriented policing is that this concept calls on police to analyze problems, which can include learning more about victims as well as offenders, and to consider carefully why they came together where they did. The interconnectedness of person, place, and seemingly unrelated events needs to be examined and documented. Then police are to craft responses that may go beyond traditional police practices … Finally, problem‐oriented policing calls for police to assess how well they are doing. Did it work? What worked, exactly? Did the project fail because they had the wrong idea, or did they have a good idea but fail to implement it properly? (Skogan & Frydl,  2004 , p. 91).

A number of studies going back to the mid‐1980s demonstrate that problem solving can be utilized to address a variety of police concerns, including fear of crime (Cordner,  1986 ), violent and property crime (Eck & Spelman,  1987 ), firearm‐related youth homicide (Kennedy, Braga, Piehl, & Waring.,  2001 ), and various forms of disorder, including prostitution and drug dealing (Capowich & Roehl,  1994 ; Eck & Spelman,  1987 ; Hope,  1994 ). As a further example of the proliferation of POP, the Center for Problem‐Oriented Policing (POP Center, https://popcenter.asu.edu/ ) documents a large number of case studies and evaluations of POP. For instance, there have been over 1,000 programs submitted for consideration for the Goldstein Award and more than 800 submissions to the Tilley Award. These submissions document the use of a wide array of problem‐solving responses to document crime, disorder and a host of other issues police are tasked with addressing, highlighting the utility of the POP model for a wide variety of problem types (see also, Scott,  2000 ; Scott & Clarke,  2020 ). As our review is focused on impacts on crime and disorder, we limit our discussion here to those outcomes.

There are also a number of experimental and other more rigorous examinations of POP. For example, a study in Jersey City, New Jersey, public housing complexes (Mazerolle, Ready, Terrill & Waring, 2000 ) found that a police problem‐solving model could be used to respond to violent and property crime in six housing complexes. In another example, Clarke and Goldstein ( 2002 ) report a POP project to reduce thefts of appliances from new home construction in Charlotte, North Carolina. Officers carefully analyzed this problem before working with construction firms to implement changes in building practices.

Two early experimental evaluations of applications of problem solving in crime hot spots (Braga et al.,  1999 ; Weisburd & Green,  1995 ) suggested POP interventions, particular those implemented in crime hot spots, could be evaluated rigorously. 2 In a randomized trial involving Jersey City violent crime hot spots, Braga et al. ( 1999 ) examined the impact of problem solving in 12 hot spots on property and violent crime. While this study tested problem‐solving approaches, it is important to note that focused police attention was brought only to the experimental locations. Accordingly, it is difficult to distinguish between the effects of bringing focused attention to hot spots and that of such focused efforts being developed using a problem‐oriented approach. The Jersey City Drug Market Analysis Experiment (Weisburd & Green,  1995 ) more directly tested the value added of problem‐solving approaches in hot spots policing. In that study, a similar number of narcotics detectives were assigned to treatment and control hot spots. Weisburd and Green ( 1995 ) compared the effectiveness of unsystematic, arrest‐oriented enforcement based on ad hoc target selection (the control group) with a treatment strategy involving analysis of assigned drug hot spots, followed by site‐specific enforcement and collaboration with landlords and local government regulatory agencies, and concluding with monitoring and maintenance for up to a week following the intervention. More recent experimental evaluations have also examined the impact of POP in crime hot spots (e.g., Braga & Bond,  2008 ; Groff et al.,  2015 ; Taylor, Koper, & Woods,  2011 ).

In sum, POP has emerged as one of the most widely accepted and widely used strategies in American policing (Scott,  2000 ; Weisburd & Majmundar,  2018 ). This is indicated both by the adoption of POP by major federal agencies and national policing groups, the creation of national awards for effective POP programs, and the widespread adoption of the approach in American policing and throughout the world. For example, the U.S. federal agency, the Office of Community Oriented Policing Services (COPS) adopted POP as a key strategy, initially funding the Center for Problem‐Oriented Policing, which has developed over 70 problem‐specific guides for police. More recently, the Bureau of Justice Assistance has also funded the creation of problem‐oriented response and tool guides. The Police Executive Research Forum adopted POP as a “powerful tool in the policing arsenal,” in the 1980s and began to run a yearly national conference to promulgate and advance POP strategies (Solé Brito & Allan,  1999 , p. xiii) that the POP Center still continues today. In 1993 the Herman Goldstein Award was created for “problem solving excellence.” In the United Kingdom, the Tilley Award for POP was created in 1999. 3 To date there have been more than 1,800 submissions to these awards. Reflecting the wide‐scale adoption of POP by American police agencies, the 2013 Law Enforcement Management and Administrative Statistics (LEMAS) survey reported that 33% of all departments, and 74% of departments serving 100,000–249,000 citizens, reported actively encouraging officer involvement in problem‐solving projects (Reaves,  2015 ). 4

While POP has been widely adopted and assessed, it is important to note that fully implementing POP has been challenging (Cordner & Biebel,  2005 ; Maguire, Uchida, & Hassell,  2015 ), and programs are often characterized by partial implementations of the SARA model that have been termed “shallow” problem‐solving (Braga & Weisburd,  2010 ). For instance, in his Stockholm lecture Goldstein ( 2018 ) noted that many early initiatives lacked fundamental understanding of the POP approach and that he did not adequately acknowledge the importance of having enough individuals with the requisite research and assessment skills when developing his model. However, he also noted that he has been impressed by improvements in areas such as focusing specifically on micro‐problems, the engagement of rank‐and‐file officers in problem solving, the use of a broad range of responses, and increasing engagement of the private sector in partnerships to share responsibility for public safety problems. Thus while POP still has a long way to go to be fully embedded in police agencies, much less to become standard police practice as Goldstein hoped, there is reason to believe that the model is both spreading and improving in quality over time.

2.2. POP in practice

Since its initial proposition, the POP model has been further articulated by Eck and Spelman ( 1987 ) whose work in Newport News produced the SARA model. SARA is an acronym representing four steps they suggest police should follow when implementing POP. “Scanning” is the first step, and involves the police identifying and prioritizing potential problems in their jurisdiction that may be causing crime and disorder. After potential problems have been identified, the next step is “Analysis.” This involves the police analyzing the identified problem(s) in‐depth using a variety of data sources so that appropriate responses can be developed. The third step, “Response,” has the police developing and implementing interventions tailored to what was learned in the “analysis” step and designed to solve the problem(s). The search for responses should be broad and not limited to law enforcement methods, and often should involve partnering with other agencies, community groups and/or community members depending on the type of problem and its causes. Indeed, the POP model stresses the need to shift and share responsibility for public safety, and this will require police to identify and mobilize partners (Goldstein,  2018 ; Scott & Goldstein,  2005 ). Finally, once the response has been administered, the final step is “Assessment” which involves assessing the impact of the response on the targeted problem(s).

For example, a police agency may determine that drug‐related crime is on the rise in their jurisdiction, constituting a problem in need of prioritization (Scanning phase). Further examination of the nature of drug‐related crime may reveal problem areas and times (Analysis phase). Based on this analysis, the agency may choose to direct increased patrol and enforcement to the specific areas deemed problematic at the specific times deemed problematic and to partner with community organizations to deliver substance abuse treatment programs (Response phase). After a period of time the agency may compare drug‐related crime in the jurisdiction as a whole, as well as in the targeted areas, from before and after the response was implemented (Assessment phase).

This process in general, rather than the specific problem or response chosen, represents the core concept of POP. Thus, a diverse set of variations in problems, responses, and length of interventions are possible across an array of targets (i.e., problem places of varying size or problem people may be the focus) and virtually any unit of analysis.

2.3. How might POP work?

It is hypothesized that POP affects change in problem outcomes through an increased knowledge of, and responsiveness to, the mechanisms through which a particular problem operates. The National Academies of Sciences Committee on Proactive Policing noted in its consensus report:

Problem‐oriented policing is an analytic method for developing crime reduction tactics. This strategy draws upon theories of criminal opportunity, such as rational choice and routine activities, to analyze crime problems and develop appropriate responses (Braga,  2008 ; Clarke,  1997 ; Reisig,  2010 ). Using a basic iterative process of problem identification, analysis, response, assessment, and adjustment of the response (often called the scanning, analysis, response, and assessment [SARA] model), this adaptable and dynamic analytic method provides a framework for uncovering the complex mechanisms at play in crime problems and for developing tailor‐made interventions to address the underlying conditions that cause crime problems (Eck & Spelman,  1987 ; Goldstein,  1990 ). Depending on the nuances of particular problems, the responses that are developed—even for seemingly similar problems—can be diverse. Indeed, problem‐oriented policing interventions draw upon a variety of tactics and practices, ranging from arrest of offenders and modification of the physical environment to engagement with community members (Weisburd & Majmundar,  2018 , p. 53).

POP is not concerned simply with the problem outcomes themselves but rather with the underlying processes that lead to problems emerging and developing. Addressing the underlying mechanisms that cause problems should lead to long‐term solutions and should lead police agencies to think and act in ways that go beyond their normal day‐to‐day operations. Furthermore, the assessment of results should lead to refinement and improvement in subsequent efforts.

Moreover, while the POP model does not favor any particular kind of intervention, one can still look beyond this literature and find support for the basic ideas behind the models by examining evidence for approaches commonly utilized in POP programs. For instance, evidence of the effectiveness of situational and opportunity‐blocking strategies, while not necessarily police based, provides indirect support for the effectiveness of problem solving in reducing crime and disorder. Moreover, POP has been linked to routine activity theory, rational choice perspectives, and situational crime prevention (Clarke,  1992a ,  1992b ; Eck & Spelman,  1987 ). Reviews of prevention programs designed to block crime and disorder opportunities in small places find that most of the studies report reductions in target crime and disorder events (Eck,  2002 ; Poyner,  1981 ; Weisburd & Telep, 2014 ; Weisburd,  1997 ), 5 and many of these efforts were the result of police problem‐solving strategies. Further, a systematic review and meta‐analysis of situational crime prevention both supports its effectiveness and that such approaches do not merely displace crime to other areas (Guerette & Bowers,  2009 ). Lastly, hot spots policing and focused deterrence approaches that involve problem‐solving have been found effective in recent systematic reviews (Braga, Turchan, Papachristos, & Hureau,  2019 ; Braga, Weisburd, & Turchan,  2019 ).

2.4. Why is it important to do this review?

This is an update to an earlier Campbell systematic review of the effectiveness of POP that included studies through 2006 and identified a total of 10 studies that met the Campbell criterion for inclusion—4 randomized experiments and 6 quasi‐experiments (Weisburd et al.,  2008 ,  2010 ). Overall, the findings of this review largely reinforced those of prior narrative reviews and more general assumptions of the effectiveness of POP. Specifically, the authors noted “[w]hether we used a more conservative mean effect size approach or examined the largest effects on crime and disorder reported, we found that POP approaches have a statistically significant effect on the outcomes examined. Importantly, the results are similar whether we look at experimental or nonexperimental studies” (Weisburd et al.,  2010 , p. 162).

However, the original review also noted that effect sizes were relatively modest, ranging between 0.10 and 0.20 (measured as Cohen's D ) and were based upon only 10 experimental or quasi‐experimental studies. As such, an updated review may help to shed further light on the ability of POP to reduce crime and disorder problems by analyzing an increased base of empirical research on POP interventions by including studies up to the end of 2018 (12 years beyond the cutoff for the original review's search). Having more complete and current evidence on POP is especially important given an increasing focus on problem‐solving and other proactive policing approaches around the world (Weisburd & Majmundar,  2008 ).

We also add an additional approach to measuring effect sizes suggested by Wilson ( in progress ) that has statistical properties better suited to the nature of place‐based data and provides a more easily interpretable set of estimates of program outcomes. As empirical knowledge on POP's effectiveness increases, police agencies may be able to better determine ways to identify and respond to the various problems occurring in their jurisdictions.

3. OBJECTIVES

The objectives of this updated review are to extend the findings of the original review (Weisburd et al.,  2008 ,  2010 ) by synthesizing the findings of published and unpublished evaluations of POP through December 2018 to assess its overall impacts on crime and disorder. Spatial displacement was also assessed for studies that provided data needed to calculate effect sizes for such effects. Finally, while too few studies included outcomes other than crime or disorder to allow for meaningful meta‐analyses, impacts on items such as police legitimacy and fear of crime are reviewed narratively, as well as findings about the financial cost/benefits of POP.

4.1. Criteria for considering studies for this review

4.1.1. types of studies.

For studies to be considered in this review the evaluation had to include a target area or group that received a POP intervention AND a control area/group that received standard police services. The control condition could be either experimental or quasi‐experimental (Campbell & Stanley, 1966 ; Cook & Campbell, 1979 ; Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002 ).

The following research designs were eligible for inclusion in our review (this is adapted from the inclusion criterion in Global Policing Database protocol [Higginson, Eggins, Mazerolle, & Stanko,  2015 , pp. 47–48]):

  • Randomized experimental designs (RCTs)
  • ◦ Regression discontinuity designs
  • ◦ Matched control group designs with or without preintervention baseline measures (propensity or statistically matched)
  • ◦ Unmatched control group designs with preintervention measures (difference‐in‐difference analysis)
  • ◦ Short interrupted time‐series designs with control group (less than 25 pre and 25 postintervention observations [Glass,  1997 ])
  • ◦ Long interrupted time‐series designs with control group (≥25 pre‐ and postintervention observations ([Glass,  1997 ])
  • ◦ Unmatched control group designs with pre–postintervention measures which allow for difference‐in‐difference analyses
  • ◦ Unmatched control group designs without preintervention measures where the control group has face validity
  • ◦ Raw unadjusted correlational designs where the variation in the level of the intervention is compared with the variation in the level of the outcome
  • ◦ Treatment‐Treatment Designs

Unlike some Campbell reviews, we included studies with nonequivalent control groups; for example, studies that compared a target area to the rest of the jurisdiction. As the POP model requires police to identify specific problems in specific areas or populations, it will often be difficult for evaluators to create equivalent comparison areas/groups (Eck,  2006a ). As such, we did not restrict our review to quasi‐experiments with equivalent control groups as we felt it important to be inclusive of studies that were representative of how POP is often carried out in practice. Thus any evaluation of POP that included a comparison group that did not receive the POP intervention was eligible for our review if it met our other inclusion criteria

4.1.2. Type of areas/groups

As noted above, POP is a general approach that calls for police to identify specific problems and develop specific responses to them based on potential underlying causes determined through problem analysis. As such, POP is not limited to any specific unit of analysis. For example, problems can be citywide, confined to small areas such as hot spots or can be individual offenders or groups of offenders rather than places. As such our review is not restricted by the type of target and includes problems at any unit of analysis that were addressed with a POP intervention.

4.1.3. Types of interventions

Given that the POP model calls for police to develop tailor‐made responses designed to address underlying causes of identified problems, a nearly limitless array of interventions can be associated with the approach. As such, our review is not restricted to any specific type of police response to crime or disorder problems. In this review we treat the use of the SARA model described above to identify problems, research underlying causes and develop and deliver specific responses to address them as the “intervention.” That is to say that our central question is whether using the SARA model to identify and respond to problems is associated with larger crime reduction compared with traditional reactive policing strategies. Further, we did not require that publications specifically mention the SARA steps (or even POP). We carefully read every potentially eligible study identified through our search and included studies if we could determine the interventions roughly followed the tenets of the SARA model. 6

4.1.4. Types of outcome measures

The primary outcomes examined in this review, and included in our meta‐analyses, are measures of crime and disorder. By far the most commonly used measures of these outcomes in evaluations of POP are police recorded calls for service or incident reports. However, all measures of crime and disorder such as arrests, social observations or resident perceptions were coded. We also coded survey measures of other outcomes such as citizen perceptions/opinions of police, fear of crime, and collective efficacy where possible. We had hoped to get enough of these types of measures to conduct meaningful meta‐analyses on some of these types of outcomes. However, few studies reported on more than crime/disorder outcomes, and those that did are characterized by wide variation in measures used and data reported in study publications. As such we provide a narrative review and summary of the limited findings for such outcomes, as well as cost‐benefit analyses. We also conducted a meta‐analysis of displacement/diffusion effects, and the narrative summaries in Appendix A also discuss conclusions about these effects drawn in studies that did not provide data needed to calculate these effect sizes.

4.2. Search strategy for identification of studies

The search for this updated review was led by the Global Policing Database (GPD) research team at the University of Queensland (Elizabeth Eggins and Lorraine Mazerolle) and Queensland University of Technology (Angela Higginson). The University of Queensland is home to the GPD (see http://www.gpd.uq.edu.au ), which served as the main search location for this review. The GPD is a web‐based and searchable database designed to capture all published and unpublished experimental and quasi‐experimental evaluations of policing interventions conducted since 1950. There are no restrictions on the type of policing technique, type of outcome measure or language of the research (Higginson et al.,  2015 ). The GPD is compiled using systematic search and screening techniques, which are reported in Higginson et al. ( 2015 ) and summarized in detail in Appendices B and C . Broadly, the GPD search protocol includes an extensive range of search locations to ensure that both published and unpublished research is captured across criminology and allied disciplines.

To capture studies, we used POP terms to search the GPD corpus of full‐text documents that have been screened as reporting on a quantitative impact evaluation of a policing intervention. Specifically, we used the following terms to search the title and abstract fields of the corpus of documents published from January 2006 through to December 2018:

  • "problem‐orient*”
  • "problem orient*"
  • “problem solv*”
  • "problem focus*"
  • “problem ident*”
  • “ident* problem*”
  • “situational crime prevent*”

Several additional strategies were also used to extend the GPD search. First, we performed forward citation searches for works that have cited seminal POP studies. 7 Second, we conducted hand searches of 2017 and 2018 volumes of leading journals in the field to identify any recent studies that may have not yet been indexed in the GPD. 8 Third, we reviewed the Center for Problem‐Oriented Policing website for all Tilley Award and Goldstein Award winners and submissions. 9 Fourth, after finishing the above searches and reviewing the studies as described later, we e‐mailed the list to leading policing scholars knowledgeable in the area of POP (see list in Appendix D ). This was aimed at identifying studies the above searches missed, as these experts may be able to refer us to eligible studies missing from our list, particularly unpublished pieces such as dissertations and smaller research reports.

Several strategies were used to obtain full‐text versions of the studies found through our search. First, we attempted to obtain full‐text versions from the electronic journals available through the George Mason University, Georgia State University, and Arizona State University libraries. When electronic versions were not available, we used print versions of journals available at the library. If the journals were not available, we made use of both the GPD team and the Interlibrary Loan Office (ILL) to obtain the journal from the libraries of other area schools. When those methods did not work, we contacted the author(s) of the article and/or the agency that conducted and/or funded the research to try to get a copy of the full‐text version of the study.

4.3. Data collection and analysis

Search results were given title and abstract review by Kevin Petersen, one of the authors of this review. Any studies that were not obviously eligible or ineligible were flagged. Flagged studies were reviewed by the other three authors of this review, who then discussed and voted on each study's eligibility. All inclusion/exclusion decisions were unanimous.

4.3.1. Details of study coding categories

All eligible studies were coded on a variety of criteria including (but not limited to):

  • a. Reference information (title, authors, publication, etc.)
  • b. Nature of description of selection of site, problems, etc.
  • c. Nature and description of selection of comparison group or period
  • d. The unit of analysis
  • e. The sample size
  • f. Methodological type (randomized experiment, quasi‐experiment, or pre–post test)
  • g. A description of the POP intervention
  • h. Dosage intensity and type
  • i. Implementation difficulties
  • j. The statistical test(s) used
  • k. Reports of statistical significance (if any)
  • l. Effect size/power (if any)
  • m. Cost‐benefit analysis (if applicable)
  • n. The conclusions drawn by the authors

The full coding sheet is provided in Appendix E . Kevin Petersen (one of the authors of the review) and another graduate research assistant at George Mason University independently coded each eligible study. Where there were discrepancies, Drs. Hinkle, Weisburd and Telep reviewed the study, had discussions and voted to determine the final coding decision.

4.3.2. Statistical procedure and conventions

We completed a meta‐analysis of the 34 eligible studies by calculating a standardized effect size for each included outcome and then estimating an overall random effect for the impact of POP on crime and disorder. We used Biostat's Comprehensive Meta Analysis 3.0 program for our analyses and to create the forest plots we present below.

Computation of effect sizes in the studies was not always direct. The goal was to convert all observed effects into a standardized mean difference effect size metric. None of the studies we examined calculated standardized effect sizes, and indeed, it was sometimes difficult to develop precise effect size metrics from published materials. This reflects a more general problem in crime and justice with “reporting validity” (Farrington,  2006 ; Lösel & Köferl,  1989 ), and has been documented in recent reviews of reporting validity in crime and justice studies (see Perry & Johnson,  2008 ; Perry, Weisburd, & Hewitt,  2010 ).

For many of our eligible studies, effect sizes could only be calculated using pre‐ and postintervention crime/disorder counts for the treatment and control group/area. A similar approach was used for some studies in our earlier review and is common in systematic reviews of policing interventions (e.g., Braga and colleagues’ [ 2019 ] recent update of their Campbell review of the effectiveness of hot spots policing).

This approach involves calculating relative incidence rate ratio (RIRR), and the variance of the log RIRR from the raw counts using the following formulae (the table provides an example of the grid of pre and post counts used for these equations):

The variance of the log of the RIRR (V(log RIRR)) was adjusted for over‐dispersion using the approach outlined by Farrington, Gill, Waples and Argomaniz ( 2007 ). 10 This adjustment is calculated as the product of V(log RIRR) and D , with D  = 0.0008 ×  N  + 1.2. N is indexed as the mean number of incidents per case and is calculated as the total number of incidents ( a  +  b  +  c  +  d ) divided by the total number of treatment plus control areas/groups.

Finally, Cohen's D is obtained by multiplying the log of RIRR by √3/ π , while its standard error is calculated by multiplying the adjusted V(log RIRR) by (3/ π 2 ; Hasselblad & Hedges,  1995 ).

While the Cohen's D approach allows us to compare our findings to the prior review, Wilson ( in progress ) argues that the Cohen's D approach fails to produce effect sizes that are comparable across studies when based on place‐based count data (the majority of studies in our review). Moreover, he has also pointed out that Cohen's D s obtained through the above conversion are not comparable to those calculated directly through conventional means. As such, we also present meta‐analysis models where the effect size is the log RIRR and its standard error (which is the square root of adjusted V(log RIRR)). 11 This approach also has an advantage in that the exponent of the log RIRR can be interpreted simply as the relative percent change in the treatment group compared with the control group.

4.3.3. Determination of independent findings

We first note that a few studies had multiple publications found through our searches. In these cases, the publication that provided the data used to calculate effect sizes was considered the main study and that is what is listed in tables, figures and the text. In cases where the effect size data were available in multiple publications, we treated the peer‐reviewed journal article as the main publication for the study (including in our coding of publication type). Secondary publications associated with the project that may have been used to help complete other items on our coding instrument are listed below the main publication in the list of eligible studies (via “see also” notes) in Section  5.1.1 . There were no cases where unique crime/disorder outcomes for our main analyses were found across publications for the same study.

A common problem in conducting meta‐analyses in crime and justice is that investigators often do not prioritize the outcomes examined. This is common in studies in the social sciences in which authors view good practice as demanding that all relevant outcomes be reported. However, the lack of prioritization of outcomes in a study raises the question of how to derive an overall effect of treatment. For example, the reporting of one significant result may reflect a type of “creaming” in which the authors focus on one significant finding and ignore the less positive results of other outcomes. However, authors commonly view the presentation of multiple findings as a method for identifying the specific contexts in which a treatment is effective. When the number of such comparisons is small and therefore unlikely to affect the error rates for specific comparisons such an approach is often valid.

This is a particularly important issue for the current review. Given that POP calls for police to identify specific problems and develop tailor‐made solutions, it is important to include only outcomes likely to have been impacted by such focused responses. For example, in the Mazerolle et al. ( 2000 ) study, the authors noted that the Beat Health program “uses a variety of tactics to resolve drug and disorder issues” (p. 220). The authors present data on calls for service for disorder, drug crime, property crime, and violent crime. Because of their description of the intervention, we chose to include only drug and disorder calls as primary outcomes, and these were the outcomes we used for our mean effect size discussed below.

A primary outcome is defined in our review as one that was the direct focus of the POP intervention. The police needed to be specifically targeting the crime or call type in an outcome for us to identify an outcome as primary. We note that we erred on the side of being inclusive and only excluding reported crime/disorder outcomes in cases where the studies made it abundantly clear that only certain reported outcomes were the direct target of the tailored POP intervention. As such, for the vast majority of studies we include all reported crime/disorder outcomes.

We also note that it is important to examine variation in impacts across outcomes, and as such we analyze the studies using two approaches. The first is conservative in the sense that it combines all relevant outcomes reported into an overall average effect size statistic for each study. Second, to provide a range of effects, we also present separate models based on the largest and smallest effect for each study with multiple included outcomes. For studies with a sole outcome, or a clearly‐specified primary outcome, the same effect size is reported in all models. We also examined the impacts of POP across crime type.

In addition to providing a range, this approach is important as in some of the studies with more than one outcome reported, the largest outcome reflected what authors thought would be the largest program effect. This was true for the Jersey City Drug Market Analysis Experiment, which examined violent and property crimes, but assumed that the largest program effects, given the nature of the intervention, would be found in the case of calls for disorder (Weisburd & Green,  1995 ).

4.3.4. Treatment of qualitative research

Qualitative research on crime and disorder outcomes was not included in this review. Our goal was to summarize the findings of experimental and quasi‐experimental evaluations of the quantitative impacts of POP on crime and disorder. Purely qualitative studies do not meet the inclusion criteria of the GPD and would not have come up in our searches. The authors encourage other researchers to examine whether there is a sufficient amount of qualitative research on POP to warrant a systematic review.

5.1. Selection of studies

5.1.1. results of the search.

Search strategies in systematic reviews return a large number of results that must be screened for eligibility. Utilizing the GPD helped keep this number more manageable. Even though the GPD search strategy is much more comprehensive than those typically employed in searches by researchers conducting individual reviews, the studies included in the database have already been screened and confirmed to be policing evaluations that meet their methodological criterion (see above and the full details provided in Appendices B and C ).

The initial steps of the review consisted of reviewing titles and abstracts to eliminate any duplicates and studies that were clearly not evaluations of POP. For any studies that could not be eliminated at this this step, we obtained the full‐text of the articles, reports, theses/dissertations or books for careful review to assess whether the interventions and evaluations met the eligibility criterion.

In total, the GPD searches and other strategies used in this review yielded a total of 2,464 results to review. Reviewing titles and abstracts eliminated 1,481 studies which were clearly not evaluations of POP. This left 983 studies which received full‐text review. Of these 39 met our eligibility criteria, and 24 provided the quantitative data needed to calculate effect sizes for our meta‐analyses. As the original Campbell systematic review of POP (Weisburd et al.,  2008 ,  2010 ) included 10 experimental or quasi‐experimental studies in their main analyses we have a total of 34 studies included in our summary tables and meta‐analyses.

Of the 983 publications which received full‐text review, 746 studies were Goldstein or Tilley Award submissions (all available award submissions from 2006 to 2018 received full‐text review), 204 studies were GPD search results, and 33 studies were identified via forward citation searches.

Figure  1 provides a visual summary of the number of eligible studies by year of publication. As the graph highlights, there was a clear increase in evaluations of POP in the years after the 2006 cutoff for our original review. This uptick was relatively evenly spread over the 12‐year period, with each year other than 2014 having between one and five eligible studies. While it is possible that some of the increase is due to the use of the GPD for searches for this update, the data suggest that there has simply been an increase in experimental and quasi‐experimental evaluations of POP since 2006. For example, the original review only included 2 Goldstein/Tilley award submissions, while our update found 11 new submissions which met our inclusion criteria and included the data needed for effect size calculations.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g001.jpg

Number of eligible problem‐oriented policing studies by year ( N  = 49)

Below we list the 49 studies that met our inclusion criterion. We first list the 34 studies which provided the quantitative data needed to be included in our meta‐analyses. We refer to these as “included studies.” As noted above, the publication that provided data for the effect size(s) for each study is listed, and any supplemental publications that were used to complete other parts of our coding of studies are listed in sub‐bullets via “see also” notes. Below this, we list the 15 studies that are not included in our analyses due to not providing sufficient data to calculate effect sizes for either of our approaches. Both lists are in chronological order.

Studies included in summary tables and meta‐analyses ( N  = 34):

  • Sherman, L., Buerger, M., & Gartin, P. ( 1989 ). Repeat call address policing: The Minneapolis RECAP Experiment . Washington, DC: Crime Control Institute.
  • Stone, S. S. ( 1993 ). Problem‐oriented policing approach to drug enforcement: Atlanta as a case study (Ph.D. dissertation). Emory University.
  • Weisburd, D., & Green, L. ( 1995 ). Policing drug hot spots: The Jersey City drug market analysis experiment. Justice Quarterly , 12 (4), 711–735.
  • Stokes, R., Donahue, N., Caron, D., & Greene, J. R. ( 1996 ). Safe travel to and from school: A problem‐oriented policing approach . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
  • San Diego Police Department. (1998). Coordinated agency network . San Diego, CA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • ◦ See also Braga ( 1997 ).
  • Mazerolle, L., Price, J. F., & Roehl, J. ( 2000 ). Civil remedies and drug control: A randomized field trial in Oakland, California. Evaluation Review, 24 (2), 212–241.
  • Knoxville Police Department. ( 2002 ). The Knoxville public safety collaborative . Knoxville, TN: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Baker, T., & Wolfer, L. ( 2003 ). The crime triangle: Alcohol, drug use, and vandalism. Police Practice and Research, 4 (1), 47–61.
  • Nunn, S., Quinet, K., Rowe, K., & Christ, D. ( 2006 ). Interdiction day: Covert surveillance operations, drugs, and serious crime in an inner‐city neighborhood. Police Quarterly , 9 (1), 73–99.
  • San Angelo Police Department. ( 2006 ). “ See! It's me!” identity theft prevention program . San Angelo, TX: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Tuffin, R., Morris, J., & Poole, A. ( 2006 ). An evaluation of the impact of the National Reassurance Policing Programme. London, UK: Home Office Research.
  • ◦ See also Reno Police Department ( 2006 ).
  • Boston Police Department. ( 2008 ). District D‐14: Breaking and entering solution plan . Boston, MA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • ◦ See also Braga and Bond ( 2009 ).
  • Lancashire Constabulary. ( 2008 ). “ Moppin” up dodge . Lancashire, UK: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Lexington Division of Police. ( 2009 ). Community law enforcement action and response program . Lexington, KY: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Vancouver Police Department. ( 2009 ). Reclaiming the “street of shame”: A problem oriented solution to Vancouver's entertainment district . Vancouver, BC: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Guseynov, N. R. ( 2010 ). Policing serious crime: A longitudinal examination of geographically focused policing activities (Master's thesis). University of Missouri‐Kansas City.
  • London Borough of Enfield. ( 2011 ). Safe as houses‐ domestic burglary project . London, UK. Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Niagara County Sheriff's Office. ( 2011 ). Operation panther pride . Lockport, NY: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Taylor, B., Koper, C. S., & Woods, D. J. ( 2011 ). A randomized controlled trial of different policing strategies at hot spots of violent crime. Journal of Experimental Criminology , 7 (2), 149–181.
  • Houston Police Department. ( 2012 ). Back from the brink: Reclaiming the Antoine corridor and the development of problem oriented policing within the Houston Police Department . Houston, TX: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Lancashire Constabulary. ( 2012 ). T he custody experience: Reducing 1st time entrants into the criminal justice system . Lancashire, UK: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • ◦ See also Chula Vista Police Department ( 2009 ).
  • Bond, B. J., & Hajjar, L. M. ( 2013 ). Measuring congruence between property crime problems and response strategies: Enhancing the problem‐solving process. Police Quarterly , 16 (3), 323–338.
  • ◦ See also Ratcliffe, Groff, Sorg, and Haberman ( 2015 ).
  • Hollywood Police Department. ( 2015 ). West district burglary reduction initiative . Hollywood, FL: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • ◦ See also Kochel and Weisburd ( 2017 , 2019 ).
  • ◦ See also White and Katz ( 2013 ) and Glendale Police Department ( 2016 ).
  • Durham Constabulary. ( 2017 ). Reducing dwelling burglaries in areas which repeatedly suffer high rates in county Durham, UK . County Durham, UK: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Zidar, M. S., Shafer, J. G., & Eck, J. E. ( 2017 ). Reframing an obvious police problem: Discovery, analysis and response to a manufactured problem in a small city. Policing: A Journal of Policy and Practice , 12 (3), 316–331.
  • Gill, C., Weisburd, D., Vitter, Z., Shader, C. G., Nelson‐Zagar, T., & Spain, L. ( 2018 ). Collaborative problem‐solving at youth crime hot posts: A pilot study. Policing: An International Journal , 41 (3), 325–338.
  • Cooley, W., Bemiller, M., Jefferis, E., & Penix, R. ( 2019 ). Neighborhood by neighborhood: Community policing in a rust belt city. Policing: An International Journal , 42 (2), 226–239. 12

Eligible studies which lacked effect size data ( N  = 15):

  • Hampshire Constabulary. ( 2006 ). Operation Mullion: Reducing anti‐social behaviour and crime in and around Mayfield School . Portsmouth, Hampshire, UK: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • South Yorkshire Police. ( 2006 ). Focusing on car crime: An initiative by South Yorkshire Police to tackle the problem of offenders stealing from Ford Focus cars . Barnsley, South Yorkshire, UK: Tilley Award Submission.
  • Charlotte‐Mecklenburg Police Department. ( 2007 ). Operation safe storage . Charlotte, NC: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Regina Police Services. ( 2007 ). Regina auto theft strategy . Saskatchewan, Canada: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Northhamptonshire Police. ( 2008 ). Northampton Countywide Traveler Unit . Northhamptonshire, UK: Tilley Award Submission.
  • Sussex Police. ( 2008 ). Operation athlete . Sussex, UK: Tilley Award Submission.
  • Anaheim Police Department. ( 2009 ). Anaheim Police Department's GRIP on gangs: Gang reduction and intervention partnership: An early gang prevention problem solving strategy . Anaheim, CA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Warwickshire Police. ( 2009 ). Trolley safe: A design based problem solving response to reduce purse thefts from shoppers in supermarkets . Warwickshire, UK: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Dayton Police Department. ( 2011 ). The urban high school disorder reduction project: Restoring safe schools and inspiring academic excellence . Dayton, OH: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • State College Police Department. ( 2011 ). Reducing crime and disorder in rental properties: An evaluation of the state college nuisance property ordinance . State College, PA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Boston Police Department. ( 2012 ). Safe street teams problem‐oriented policing initiative. Boston, MA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Palm Beach County Sheriff's Office. ( 2012 ). Smart Policing Initiative: Increasing police legitimacy and reducing victimization against immigrants in Lake Worth . Lake Worth, FL: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Wolfe, S. E., Rojek, J., Kaminski, R., & Nix, J. ( 2015 ). City of Columbia (SC) Police Department Smart Policing Initiative: Final Report . Retrieved from http://strategiesforpolicinginnovation.com/sites/default/files/2015_Wolfe%20et%20al_SPI_Final%20Report_Submission%20to%20CNA%20and%20BJA.pdf
  • Portland Police Bureau. ( 2018 ). Zombie houses: The Portland approach to vacant homes . Portland, OR: Herman Goldstein Award Submission.
  • Carson, J. V., & Wellman, A. P. ( 2018 ). Problem‐oriented policing in suburban low‐income housing: A quasi‐experiment. Police Quarterly, 21 (2), 139–170.

Several studies that received full‐text review after the initial abstract screening were excluded after determining that they did not meet our inclusion criteria. These studies are noted in Appendix F .

5.2. Characteristics of selected studies

Table  1 provides an overview of the 34 studies that are included in our meta‐analyses. In terms of location, 28 studies (82.4%) were conducted in the United States, 5 (14.7%) in the United Kingdom and 1 (2.9%) in Canada. Studies were conducted in a total of 23 U.S. cities and 2 counties across 17 states. Lowell, MA, Jersey City, NJ and Philadelphia, PA all served as the jurisdiction for two studies. The U.K. studies included a total of eight jurisdictions, with Lancashire Constabulary serving as a study site in three evaluations. Vancouver was the setting of the Canadian study.

Characteristics of included problem‐oriented policing interventions ( N  = 34)

The study documents we identified were predominantly peer‐reviewed journal articles ( N  = 13, 38.2%) and submissions to the Goldstein Award ( N  = 13, 38.2%) for excellence in POP (no Tilley Award submissions met our inclusion criteria). There were also 4 (11.8%) research reports, 2 (5.9%) doctoral dissertations, and 2 (5.9%) master's theses. A few scholars served as an author or coauthor on multiple included studies, including David Weisburd (four studies and a coauthor of this review), Lorraine Mazerolle (three studies), Anthony Braga (two studies), and Brenda Bond (two studies).

In terms of rigor of research design, our sample of studies includes 9 (26.5%) randomized experiments and 25 (73.5%) quasi‐experiments. This is an increase from four randomized experiments and six quasi‐experiments in the original review and suggests a trend toward more rigorous evaluation of POP since 2006.

Turning to the unit of analysis for the POP interventions in these studies, 26 (76.5%) programs targeted problem places, 4 (11.8%) were targeted at place managers, and 4 (11.8%) targeted individuals. Three of the individual‐focused programs targeted problem offenders, while one intervention was aimed at potential victims. Eight studies of place‐based POP approaches quantitatively assessed displacement and diffusion effects, and we conduct a meta‐analysis on these effects.

Table  2a provides a quick overview of the type of problems targeted and the type of responses delivered, while Table  2b provides a detailed summary of the studies based on type/depth of scanning for problems and problem analyses used, the responses delivered and the research design. The latter columns in the table also highlight implementation problems and research design limitations where applicable. These are discussed in detail in the following two sections.

Targeted problems and delivered responses in problem‐oriented policing experiments and quasi‐experiments

SARA model characteristics of problem‐oriented policing experiments and quasi‐experiments

Note: The descriptions provided are summaries and are not intended to cover every aspect of the intervention.

Finally, 15 of 34 (44.1%) studies reported significant reductions in at least one crime or disorder outcome, while another 17 (50%) studies reported raw differences favoring the treatment group for at least one crime or disorder outcome. Table  3 provides a summary of study conclusions about impacts on crime and disorder, as well as displacement and diffusion of crime control benefits where applicable.

Impacts of problem‐oriented policing on crime and disorder outcomes and displacement/diffusion

5.3. Study implementation

While there was a relative lack of substantial complications reported, 67.6% ( n  = 23) of the 34 included studies did identify some degree of difficulty during implementation. Based on our coding criteria, the severity of these issues was classified as either minor ( n  = 15), more substantial ( n  = 7), or major ( n  = 1). There also appeared to be thematic consistencies across the studies in the types of issues reported. While such issues are in no way limited to POP interventions, this is perhaps indication that POP interventions are at greater risk of certain complications. Brief summaries of each study's implementation issues are also presented in Table  2b .

Given that POP is an iterative process, such that problems and responses are often continually changing, implementation issues may arise from problem instability. In the Philadelphia Policing Tactics experiment, Groff et al. ( 2015 ) note that nearly half of the POP intervention hot spots began focusing on nonviolent crime problems after determining that violent crime was no longer the primary concern in the area. Specifically, eight POP sites were noted to have targeted nonviolent or quality of life offenses, and at least four POP sites were noted to have focused on drug crime in addition to a violent or nonviolent crime problem. This is an issue from an evaluation standpoint as violent crime incidents were the only measured outcomes; thus it is possible that the interventions were effective in ameliorating the problems that they targeted, but this was unclear absent measurement of those outcomes. The RECAP experiment (Sherman et al.,  1989 ) also suffered from issues related to problem instability. Specifically, call trends for many high‐crime addresses were remarkably heterogeneous from year to year, subsequently reducing the experiment's statistical power. The RECAP experiment and the Philadelphia Policing Tactics experiment both suffered from additional resource constraints as well. In the Philadelphia Policing Tactics experiment, POP officers were not dedicated to the intervention full‐time, but were instead drawn from patrol and expected to conduct POP activities during their free time. Similarly, in the RECAP experiment, there were likely too many addresses assigned to the experimental unit, thus spreading the unit's resources too thin and, perhaps, contributing to the lack of effectiveness in the second half of the intervention year.

The reality of resource constraints and other internal barriers to proper program implementation was not uncommon across these studies. Stokes ( 1996 ) reported that the safe travel corridor was poorly staffed during the afternoon hours, despite violence being more prevalent during this time. It was subsequently determined that this incongruence was due to officer shift changes and high numbers of outside calls during the afternoon hours. These factors created a gap in coverage and limited police resources toward the intervention (though the authors note that officer presence was adequate due to Temple University Police presence). It was also revealed that very few students were aware of the safety corridor, though it is not clear whether this was resource related, as school administration reported that the corridor was advertised over school announcements and letters that were distributed to students and parents. Stone ( 1993 ) also reported organizational and resource‐related constraints during the Atlanta public housing POP project. There was a relative lack of interest regarding the intervention within the department, little administrative support, and police training was minimal. These issues were compounded by the fact that the city of Atlanta had hosted the Democratic National Convention prior to the intervention, which forced officers to delay vacation during this time. Thus, when the POP project started, many officers opted to take time off and the project was chronically understaffed. In their evaluation of the Lowell (MA) Smart Policing Initiative (SPI, now referred to as Strategies for Police Innovation), Bond and Hajjar ( 2013 ) also noted that a common complaint from police captains was a lack of resources. It was suggested that such constraints prevented an increased level of proactivity during the intervention, though these issues appeared to be minor as the intervention was still considered to be effective. Minor issues with internal resistance were also noted by the Knoxville Police Department ( 2002 ) during their Public Safety Collaborative.

Police subversion concerns created barriers for both the Jersey City violent places POP study (Braga,  1997 ; Braga et al.,  1999 ) and the Drug Market Analysis Program (Weisburd & Green,  1995 ). Partly as a result of officer resistance, the Drug Market Analysis Program achieved limited implementation in the first 9 months, with only nine hot spots receiving all program elements. This forced Weisburd and Green ( 1995 ) to increase the length of the intervention and develop a more detailed implementation schedule, and ultimately the program was fully implemented for the last 5 months of the intervention period. Braga ( 1997 ) noted similar resistance among officers in the violent places POP project, as well as a disconnect between middle management and department headquarters that threatened the integrity of the program and slowed progress during the first 8 months. Ultimately the intervention unit was placed under new leadership and protocols were established to document instances of subversion. Braga also noted significant organizational changes, such as an influx of retirement and scheduled vacations which strained resources and reduced the sample size of the experiment.

Another frequent implementation barrier was resistance from stakeholders that were intended to be involved in the intervention. In the Glendale SPI study, White and Katz ( 2013 ) indicated that in phase I of the response, the SPI team was largely unsuccessful in working with Circle K management to change the physical structure and operating policies of the stores. They note that, at this stage, communication between Circe K representatives and the SPI team suffered, and the intervention was forced in a different direction. Partly as a result of this, Dario ( 2016 ) expressed concern over treatment dosage, noting that treatment quality likely varied by store location due, in part, to differing levels of responsiveness. In their attempted intervention with Walmart management, Zidar et al. ( 2017 ) also reported resistance toward environmental and policy‐oriented intervention measures. This resistance dictated the future direction of the program, as the initial plan to partner with Walmart seemed futile. In response, Zidar et al. forced responsibility on Walmart leadership by forcing them to handle petit shoplifting incidents without police assistance; however, even after doing so they noted instances of Walmart loss‐prevention misrepresenting case facts to illicit police response.

The San Angelo Police Department ( 2006 ) cited heavy opposition from business owners to the implementation of an identification checking program, largely over concern that the program would inconvenience customers. This resistance forced the department to shift responsibility for the intervention toward the customer, though despite this, few businesses ever became willing to implement the program. The Motel Interdiction Team (MIT) program in Reno experienced similar complications (Elliott,  2007 ; Reno Police Department,  2006 ). Motel owners were concerned about the economic ramifications that would result from the eviction of criminal tenants. The Reno Police Department ( 2006 ) noted that it became difficult to educate these owners about recognition of criminal behavior and eviction processes, ultimately slowing the intervention's progress. In response to uncooperative property owners, the Houston Police Department ( 2012 ) increased code enforcement in their targeted intervention of the Antoine corridor. However, this response temporarily led to a backlog in the court system, and prosecutors began dropping charges (though this issue was subsequently resolved by use of specialized prosecutors). Lancashire Constabulary ( 2008 ) noted a similar delay in court processing based on their enforcement responses.

Several studies reported resistance from other outside stakeholders such as neighborhood residents, community organizations, and local government. Cooley et al. ( 2019 ) described an attempted POP replication in Canton, OH. However, while the initial intervention was successful in establishing partnerships with community residents and neighborhood groups, the attempted replication was not. Cooley and colleagues noted that there was a lack of community organizations available to partner with and that local residents were distrusting of and unwilling to cooperate with police. Local community meetings were unsuccessful at bridging the gap between law enforcement and neighborhood residents, officer morale was low, and ultimately the intervention showed limited effectiveness. Issues were encountered forming resident partnerships in the St. Louis County Hot Spots in Residential Areas experiment as well (Kochel & Weisburd,  2017 ). Specifically, program evaluation revealed that resident partnerships were less frequently established than had been originally intended. While residents appeared to be cooperative in the early stages of the Brightwood Interdiction project, the same residents were subsequently caught tipping off offenders to police surveillance during the intervention (Nunn et al.,  2006 ). This slowed the evidence gathering process, but police were ultimately able to generate enough evidence to execute the planned warrant sweep. Lastly, as a result of varying levels of difficulty partnering with the community, Tuffin et al. ( 2006 ) reported that only two of six intended sites receiving full implementation, though the sites that did achieve full implementation showed strong results.

Gill et al. ( 2018 ) noted several aspects of the collaborative problem‐solving intervention in Seattle (WA) that were halted by local government resistance. In one intervention area, officers sought to implement a smoking ban, but were unable to do so largely due to a lack of political support. In another target area, officers were unable to implement environmental changes due to a city redevelopment plan that was operating on a different timeline. Gill et al. also noted that, despite the project's intention to be fully non‐enforcement, officers in one of the intervention areas felt that enforcement measures were necessary to stabilize the area.

Studies that targeted residential burglary incidents reported minor issues with environmental changes to the target area. London Borough of Enfield ( 2011 ) referenced issues with the installation of alley gates. Installation required 100% approval from area residents; however, several properties were rentals with out of town owners. The police were subsequently able to adjust the approval rate from 100% to 98% to circumvent this issue. Durham Constabulary ( 2017 ) noted similar issues installing security measures in private housing areas; however, they were eventually able to gain permission to do so. Lancashire Constabulary ( 2008 ) also documented issues implementing situational responses that altered the physical environment, and the Hollywood Police Department ( 2015 ) determined that closing access to alleyways was not financially feasible (however, they promoted the use of see‐through fencing instead).

Finally, there were some problems unique to certain studies. Sherman et al. ( 1989 ) encountered issues with hot spot selection, discovering that up to 15% of calls were “mirrors,” or duplicates created as a result of multiple 911 calls for the same incident. Additionally, several addresses that were originally believed to be independent were subsequently determined to correspond to the same building. This led to the inclusion of some buildings in both treatment and control groups, and required a series of pairwise deletions to modify the initial assignments. Bichler et al. ( 2013 ) expressed some concern over the possibility of unknown history effects, noting that at least one motel location was disqualified from their study after being the target of another policing intervention. Ultimately, however, there was no reported evidence to suggest that similar issues occurred at other intervention motels.

It is also worth noting that these studies may vary in their level of reporting validity. All interventions are likely to encounter obstacles; however, it is not necessarily the case that all such obstacles are accurately documented. At times there may be incentive to represent an intervention in the best possible light, perhaps at the expense of complete transparency. While this is certainly true of any form of research, it bears reminding that our determinations are limited to what was reported in study publications.

5.4. Risk of bias in included studies

Five main measures from our coding instrument (see Appendix E ) were used to assess potential sources of bias in our included studies. These items included: (a) Were any sources of nonequivalence or bias reported or implied in the application of the intervention or its analysis (i.e., threats to internal validity)? (b) If yes, what sources of nonequivalence or bias were identified? (c) Did the researcher(s) express any concerns over the quality of the data? (d) If yes, explain. (e) If a quasi‐experiment, how was matching of groups achieved? The studies that reported issues along these dimensions and/or compared treatment groups to the rest of a jurisdiction or population are presented in Table  4 . The remainder of our included studies reported no such issues and/or employed higher quality matching procedures.

Assessment of risk of bias in eligible problem‐oriented policing studies

Overall, only 14.7% ( n  = 5) of studies reported internal validity concerns and 5.9% ( n  = 2) of studies reported concerns over data quality. However, the validity of the matching techniques used in our sample of studies does need to be considered, as 40.0% (10 of 25) of the identified quasi‐experiments used the rest of a jurisdiction or population not receiving treatment as the comparison unit. Additionally, the remaining quasi‐experimental evaluations exclusively matched comparison units based on descriptive and demographic characteristics, or simple statistical tests of such characteristics. None of the included quasi‐experimental evaluations reported propensity or regression‐based matching techniques.

Of the randomized experiments ( n  = 9) included in this review, very few reported concerns over randomization procedures or other issues related to internal validity and bias. All experimental studies included some form of blocking or pair‐matching technique in addition to randomization. However, specific concerns were noted in a few of these experiments. In the RECAP experiment (Sherman et al.,  1989 , p. 17), there was possible contamination (or “spillover” effects) as treatment and control addresses were, at times, under shared ownership. Moreover, the instability in the call frequencies of particular addresses created additional variability between treatment and control groups. However, ultimately the groups were reported to be roughly equivalent. Despite the use of matched‐pair techniques in the Collaborative Problem‐Solving at Youth Crime Hot Spots study, the treatment and control locations were unable to be optimally matched, and Gill et al. ( 2018 ) noted some concern over the equivalence of these matched pairs.

The risks of bias in the quasi‐experimental evaluations are undoubtedly greater. However, of the 15 studies that matched treatment to comparison locations based on simple analysis of descriptive, social, or demographic characteristics, no major concerns were reported. It should be noted, however, that few of these studies ( n  = 4) were identified as providing a visual comparison of descriptive statistics between treatment and comparison areas. The remaining studies ( n  = 11) described the rationale and/or process for selection of comparison units, but did not provide further evidence that equivalence was attained.

The most notable concerns among our included studies were related to the use of particularly nonequivalent comparison groups. There were six studies that compared small geographic treatment areas (or collections of small geographic treatment areas) to city, district, or other population‐wide trends (Boston Police Department,  2008 ; Guseynov, 2010 ; Houston Police Department,  2012 ; Lexington Division of Police,  2009 ; London Borough of Enfield,  2011 ; White & Katz,  2013 ). Of note, four of these studies are Goldstein Award submissions, and while they do not report substantial concern over the comparability of the units, there are clear threats to internal validity caused by the use of such comparisons. There were another five studies that compared treatment units to the remainder of a population not receiving treatment, but where the size discrepancy between the groups was not as large (Baker & Wolfer,  2003 ; Elliott,  2007 ; San Angelo Police Department,  2006 ; Zidar et al.,  2017 ).

In addition to the inherent threat to internal validity, several of the weaker quasi‐experimental studies reported unique issues. Comparison of descriptive statistics for the treatment and control areas in the CSTAR intervention (Guseynov, 2010 ) indicated statistically significant differences on measures of race, unemployment, poverty, single parent households, and population mobility. Both Guseynov ( 2010 ) and Elliott ( 2007 ) also reported data‐related concerns. Guseynov specifically notes that the Kansas City Police Department switched reporting systems during the beginning of the postintervention period. This switch resulted in the omission of 17 weeks of data, possibly leading to bias in the analysis. Zidar et al. ( 2017 ) suggested that the significant decline in reported shoplifting incidents indicated by their evaluation may have been the result of under‐reporting rather than true changes in the outcome. The intervention involved the implementation of a new reporting system for target Walmarts only. Thus, they imply that it is likely the observed outcome was the result of this measurement change rather than changes in the incident rate. In the Glendale SPI (Dario,  2016 ; White & Katz,  2013 ), the intervention locations were selected based on high pre‐existing crime rates. Dario ( 2016 , p. 99) noted the inherent potential of regression to the mean when treatment units are selected in such a way, noting the possibility of bias in treatment selection.

5.5. Meta‐analysis of the effects of POP on crime and disorder

Our first meta‐analytic model presents the overall mean effects for 70 outcomes across the 34 included studies. As noted, above, many studies reported on multiple crime/disorder outcomes and for many the authors did not specify any one outcome as the primary target of their intervention. In our data, 13 of the 34 (38.2%) studies fit into this category. To avoid any “creaming” of results, we include all relevant outcomes from such studies. For studies with multiple outcomes, a mean effect size is used in this model; thus each study is only counted once in the analysis. This is the same approach used in our original review, as well as other recent Campbell reviews of policing strategies (e.g., Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ).

The results from the first model are presented in Figures  2a (Cohen's D model) and 2b (RIRR model). The forest plots show the standardized mean differences and log RIRRs, respectively, between the treatment and control groups, with the lines on either side representing the 95% confidence interval (CI). Effects to the right of 0 are supportive of reductions in crime/disorder, while effects to the left would suggest backfire effects where problems increased in the treatment areas relative to the controls. A random effects model was estimated based on an a priori assumption of a heterogeneous distribution of effect sizes (and the Q statistics for our models confirm this assumption).

(a) Combined effect size for study outcomes: (a) Cohen's D (random effects model, Q  = 165.177, df  = 33, p  < .001, I 2  = 80.021) and (b) Log RIRR (random effects model, Q  = 218.963, df  = 32, p  < .001, I 2  = 85.386). CI, confidence interval

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g002.jpg

The overall mean effect size for Cohen's D approach is 0.183 ( p  < .001). The largest effects were found in the studies conducted by the London Borough Enfield (0.841), Thomas (0.771), the Knoxville Police Department (0.664) and the San Angelo Police Department (0.654)—all four were submissions for the Goldstein Award. Three studies reported negative overall effects, Stone (−0.001), Taylor et al. (−0.012), and Stokes et al. (−0.203).

The overall mean effect is considered a small effect by conventional standards developed by Cohen ( 1988 ). However, Lipsey ( 1990 ) describes effects in this range as small but meaningful impacts that could “easily be of practical significance” (Lipsey, 1990 , p. 109). It is also important to note here that the studies clustered immediately around the mean effect size are randomized experimental evaluations of place‐based versions of POP in which the authors reported notable reductions in a variety of crime and disorder outcomes. (e.g., Weisburd & Green,  1995 ; ES = 0.147, Sherman et al., 1989 ; ES = 0.192; Braga & Bond,  2008 ; ES = 0.206; Braga et al.,  1999 ; ES = 0.233). In this regard, Wilson ( in progress ) has raised strong concerns in interpreting place‐based effect sizes similarly to person‐based effect sizes. While we follow standard practice here in reporting effect sizes, we think that caution should be used in interpretation of what magnitudes mean.

For example, the largest impact in the study by Nunn et al. ( 2006 ) was an effect size of 0.200 for robbery calls for service (see Figure  3 ). This is an effect at the criterion of 0.20 set by Cohen ( 1988 ) for a small effect. Looking at the raw changes in the data, we see that the relative actual reduction in the proportion of crime in the treatment condition was 30.4% percent, while the control area saw no change in robbery calls. Similarly, in Gill et al.'s ( 2018 ) study the largest impact was on calls for service in the retail treatment site. Our effect size for this outcome of 0.187 is a bit below the 0.20 threshold, yet the raw change shows a 10.3% decrease in calls compared with a 25.9% increase in the comparison areas. Our point is that standard small effect sizes may translate to very meaningful crime prevention outcomes at places.

Largest effect size for each study: (a) Cohen's D (random effects model, Q   = 489.197, df   =  33, p  < .001, I 2  = 93.254) and (b) Log RIRR (random effects model, Q  = 246.548, df  = 32, p  < .001, I 2  = 87.021). CI, confidence interval

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g019.jpg

As noted above, David Wilson ( in progress ) has argued that Cohen's D fails to produce effect sizes that are comparable across studies when based on place‐based count data and has also shown that the process of converting RIRRs to Cohen's D is problematic. As such, we also present meta‐analyses for all of our models using the Log RIRR as the effect sizes. These analyses are of 33 of 34 included studies (and 69/70 outcomes). The study by Stone ( 1993 ) is not included in the RIRR models as the data and methods used did not allow us to calculate an RIRR. This is not a major concern as this study is a near zero effect in Cohen's D model ( D  = −0.001, p  = .992). The overall RIRR model is shown in Figure  2b .

The results of the RIRR show an overall effect of 0.291. This can be interpreted as a relative present change by taking the exponent of the effect (which is a log RIRR) and then subtracting 1 from that value and multiplying by 100. For the overall model this shows that there was a 33.8% reduction in crime/disorder in the POP treatment areas/groups relative to the controls. Some caution is needed in interpreting this effect size as analyses presented below show smaller (though still positive and statistically significant) effects in randomized experiments and after accounting for publication bias. Nonetheless, this finding supports our illustration above of how Cohen's D often understates the magnitude of effects for place‐based studies (the majority of our sample) and provides further evidence that Wilson ( in progress ) is correct that Cohen's D is not the most appropriate effect size for these types of studies. We continue to present Cohen's D models throughout to allow for comparison to our prior review.

Regarding the meaning of effect sizes, we think it important to also note here that scholars have argued that approaches like POP that alter the characteristics of high‐crime places may reduce more crime in the long run than approaches such as temporarily increased police presence or crackdowns (Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ; Braga & Weisburd,  2010 ). This is the case as lasting changes to places made through identifying and solving problems may reduce crime over the long term through reducing opportunities for crime or other mechanisms (see above). Thus finding a relative reduction of 33.8% may be suggestive of even larger impacts on crime/disorder in targeted areas in the long run.

Moving beyond the overall effect size, perhaps the most striking finding is that the overall trend of mean effect sizes per study skews very heavily toward studies that produced findings in the direction of POP being effective. Specifically, 31 out of 34 studies (91.2%) in Cohen's D model have positive effect sizes, with 13 of them (38.2%) being statistically significant effects in favor of the treatment group. In the RIRR model, 30/33 studies (90.9%) reported positive impacts and 12 (36.4%) were statistically significant. Only one study (Stokes et al., 1996 ) had a statistically significant backfire effect, and that was a project that was plagued by implementation and research design limitations as reviewed in Section  5.3 . Excluding this study from our overall model slightly increases the mean effect size from 0.183 to 0.195 in Cohen's D model and increased the relative reduction in crime from 33.8% to 36.6% in the RIRR model.

Before moving on, it is also important to recall that we had 15 eligible studies that are not included in any of our analyses as they lacked data needed to calculate effect sizes. Goldstein and Tilley Award submissions accounted for 13 of these 15 studies. As one would expect for programs submitted for award consideration, all 13 of these studies discuss findings favorable to POP's effectiveness. The other two studies (Carson & Wellman,  2018 ; Wolfe et al.,  2015 ) failed to find evidence of positive impacts, but also did not note any backfire effects and pointed to challenges with fully implementing the interventions. As such, there is no reason to believe that the absence of these studies from our meta‐analyses would alter any of our conclusions.

Given that the overall models present mean effect sizes for studies reporting on multiple outcomes without a clear primary outcome specified, we felt it important to also estimate models including only the largest and smallest effect sizes for each study. For studies with a single outcome, or a clearly stated primary outcome, their effect sizes remain the same in all models. This approach provides an upper and lower bounds for the overall standardized mean effect. Figure  3a,b presents the largest effects models and Figures  4a , ​ ,b b the smallest effects models.

Smallest effect size for each study: (a) Cohen's D (random effects model, Q   = 194.992, df   =  33, p  < .001, I 2  = 83.076) and (b) Log RIRR (random effects model, Q  = 232.501, df   = 32, p  < .001, I 2  = 86.237). CI, confidence interval

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g021.jpg

Following the logic of inclusion, the overall random effect is larger when only including the outcome with the largest effect size for each study. While the mean effects Cohen's D model had an overall effect of 0.183, the largest effects model has an overall standardized mean effect of 0.271 ( p  < .001)—an increase of 0.088 (48.1%). Similarly, for the RIRR model the mean effects approach had an overall effect of 0.291 (33.7% relative reduction), the largest effects model had an overall effect of 0.357. This corresponds to a 42.9% relative reduction of crime/disorder in the treatment group.

Turning to the smallest effects, we see a lower bound for the standardized mean effect of 0.135 ( p  < .001)—a decrease of 0.048 (−26.2%) from the overall mean effect model. For the RIRR model we see a lower bound of the overall effect of 0.223, corresponding to a 25.0% relative reduction in crime/disorder. We think that this approach gives a sense of the range of effects that can be expected in POP programs. Specifically, using the RIRR results, the overall effect ranges from a 25% relative reduction when using the smallest effects to 42.9% relative reduction when using the largest effects. Importantly, our overall conclusion about the effectiveness of POP remains consistent. Regardless of the type of effect size and whether we examine the overall mean effect or look at the largest or smallest effect size, the results suggest that POP has a significant meaningful effect (Lipsey,  1990 ) in reducing crime/disorder.

Finally, we felt it important to examine variation in effect sizes across crime types. Table  5 summarizes the mean effects sizes for violent crime, property crime and disorder offenses for both our Cohen's D model and the RIRR approach. Studies that reported on aggregated crime counts that included more than one of these categories are not included in these analyses, nor are other types of outcomes that did not fit into those groupings (and lacked enough cases to perform a meaningful meta‐analysis).

The effects of problem‐oriented policing on specific crime types—mean effects

The results show that while POP had significant impacts on property crime (31.0% relative reduction) and disorder offenses (18.9% relative reduction), the overall effect for violent crime did not reach statistical significance ( p  = .156 in the RIRR model, p  = .218 in Cohen's D model). However, the effect is still in the positive direction (9.5% relative reduction) and 13 of the 18 violent crime outcomes were positive. Future research should further explore the potential reasons for the heterogeneous impact of POP across crime types.

5.5.1. Moderator analyses

We also conducted moderator analyses to examine heterogeneity in effect sizes across three dimensions—(a) experiments versus quasi‐experiments, (b) studies with nonequivalent groups versus all others, and, (c) the type of publication (scholarly publications vs. Goldstein Award submissions).

Study design is important to assess as it is well known that more rigorous designs are more likely to produce null findings (Rossi,  1987 ). Figure  5a , ​ ,b b shows the moderator results for randomized experiments versus quasi‐experiments.

Research design as a moderator for study outcomes. (a) Cohen's D . Random effects model, quasi‐experiments: Q  =  160.384, df  = 24, p  < .001; randomized experiments: Q  = 4.773, df  = 8, p  < .782; between groups: Q  = 4.914, df  = 1, p  = .027. While the Q statistic is not significant in the randomized experiments model, the random effects and fixed effect model results are identical for this subsample. (b) Log RIRR. Random effects model, quasi‐experiments: Q  = 203.223, df  = 23, p  < .001; randomized experiments: Q  = 12.186, df  = 8, p  < .143; between groups: Q  = 14.171, df  = 1, p  = .001. While the Q statistic is not significant in the randomized experiments model, the random effects and fixed effect model results are very similar for this subsample). CI, confidence interval

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g023.jpg

The moderator analysis of the impact of study design shows that quasi‐experiments have a larger overall effect size than randomized experiments. For Cohen's D model (see Figure  5a ) the quasi‐experimental studies have an effect size of 0.212 ( p  < .001), while the randomized experiments have an effect size of 0.107 ( p  < .001). The difference between groups was statistically significant ( Q  = 4.914, df  = 1, p  = .027) and the moderated effect size is 0.147 ( p  < .001). Turning to the RIRR models (see Figure  5b ) we see the same pattern. For quasi‐experiments, the effect of 0.377 (a relative reduction of 45.8%) was larger than the effect of 0.089 (a relative reduction of 9.3%) for randomized experiments. The difference between groups was again statistically significant ( Q  = 14.171, df  = 1, p  < .001) and the moderated effect size was 0.183—a relative reduction of 20.1%.

These results are consistent with Weisburd, Lum, and Yang's ( 2003 ) proposal that experimental designs more generally show smaller impacts in crime and justice research (see also, Welsh,  2016 ). The results show that while there is a bias toward finding stronger effects in studies with weaker research designs, the overall finding of a significant meaningful effect for POP is supported across study types, as well as by the moderated effect size.

Our second methodological moderator analysis examined the impact of the nonequivalent research designs highlighted in Table  4 . Specifically, we compared the studies that are listed in Table  4 as having nonequivalent control groups to studies with better matching methods. The results here suggest that studies with nonequivalent control groups did not bias our conclusions. Indeed, effect sizes were actually slightly larger in the studies with better matching methods. For Cohen's D models, the 11 studies with nonequivalent control groups had an overall effect size of 0.178 ( p  < .001), while the effect for the 23 studies with more rigorous matching approaches was 0.190 ( p  < .001). The difference between groups was not statistically significant ( Q  = 0.034, df  = 1, p  = 0.854) and the moderated effect size was 0.184 ( p  < .001). Similarly, for the RIRR models, the nonequivalent control groups had an overall effect size of 0.263 ( p  < .001; a 30.1% relative reduction), while the studies which used better matching methods had an effect of 0.309 ( p  < .001; a relative reduction of 36.2%). The between‐groups difference was again not statistically significant ( Q  = 0.231, df  = 1, p  = .631) and the moderated effect size was 0.289 ( p  < .001; a 33.5% relative reduction).

Next, we examined the impact of the type of publication. This is important as the award submissions are inherently biased toward successful outcomes and likely also toward larger effects. The reasoning here is simple—police departments are not going to submit a program that did not work for consideration for an award and are probably most likely to submit when a project has a larger impact. As such, our moderator analysis here compares the mean effects for the award submissions to those for scholarly publications (journal articles, research reports, theses and dissertations in our current sample). These results are shown in Figure  6a , ​ ,b b .

Publication type as a moderator for study outcomes (a) Cohen's D (random effects model, award submissions: Q  = 52.115, df  = 12, p  < .001; scholarly publications: Q  = 80.200, df  = 20, p  < .001; between groups: Q  = 13.702; df  = 1; p  < .001) and (b) Log RIRR (random effects model, award submissions: Q  = 58.089, df  = 12, p  < .001; scholarly publications: Q  = 113.161, df  = 19, p  < .001; between groups: Q  = 12.329; df  = 1; p  < .001). CI, confidence interval

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g025.jpg

As expected, the award submissions have a larger overall mean effect size than scholarly publications. Examining Cohen's D model (Figure  6a ) we see that the award submissions had an overall effect of 0.362 ( p  < .001) while the effect for scholarly publications was 0.101 ( p  = .001). The difference between groups was statistically significant ( Q  = 13.702, df  = 1, p  < .001) and the moderated effect size is 0.149 ( p  < .001). The results from the RIRR model (Figure  6b ) are very similar, with the effect for award submissions being 0.580 (a 78.6% relative reduction) compared with an effect of .166 (an 18.1% relative reduction) for the scholarly publications. The between‐groups difference was again statistically significant ( Q  = 12.329, df  = 1, p  < .001) and the moderated effect size was 0.228 (a 25.6% relative reduction).

These results raise possible concerns regarding the exclusion of police‐initiated POP programs that were evaluated in some way but were not submitted for award nominations. We conduct analyses regarding publication bias below, and note the biases there. Nonetheless, because our analyses without the award submissions remain statistically significant, this finding does not alter our overall conclusion of a significant crime prevention outcome for POP. In turn, these award studies do provide additional information about successful interventions and a sense of the upper range of POP impacts that are to be expected.

As a final summary and sensitivity analysis, Tables  6a and  6b below summarize the main effect sizes outlined above, and also present results from models using the smallest and largest effects in each applicable study for randomized experiments, quasi‐experiments, award submissions and scholarly publications that were not presented above to save space. The range of effects support our conclusion of a meaningful effect of POP in reducing crime in all but two of the models. The overall effect is not significant in the two most conservative models—smallest effect outcomes for randomized experiments and scholarly publications—for both Cohen's D and RIRR models. All the other models produced statistically significant overall effects. For Cohen's D models (see Table  6a ), those effects ranged from 0.101 (mean effect for randomized experiments) to 0.415 (largest effect outcomes for award submissions). The mean effects shown in the “Combined ES” column provide the overall summary of effects across all outcomes for each model and show that across all models the average overall effect of POP on crime/disorder ranges from 0.101 to 0.362. Similarly, looking at the RIRR models summarized in Table  6b (again excluding the smallest effects for the randomized experiments and scholarly publications which are not significant) shows that crime in the POP group relative to the controls was reduced between 9.3% (mean effect for randomized experiments) and 81.5% (largest effect for award submissions). The mean effect ranged from the 9.3% relative reduction in randomized experiments to 78.6% for award submissions.

Summary of effect sizes ranges across models (Cohen's D )

Summary of effect sizes ranges across models (Log RIRR)

As such, our review provides strong and consistent evidence that POP is an effective approach to reducing crime and disorder. However, there is a great deal of heterogeneity in the magnitude of effect sizes across factors such as study type, study rigor, and crime type. This will be discussed more in the discussion and conclusion sections of this report.

5.5.2. Meta‐analysis of displacement and diffusion effects

Many of our studies were place‐based approaches to POP that may have had potential to either displace crime/disorder to surrounding areas or to see the benefits of the intervention diffuse to areas that were not targeted (see Weisburd et al.,  2006 ). Eight of our studies provided data for a total of 19 outcomes that allowed us to create effect sizes and conduct a meta‐analysis of displacement and diffusion effects.

These effect sizes were calculated using pre‐ and postintervention counts for target, control, and buffer areas. This was done following the approach used by Telep, Weisburd, Gill, Vitter, and Teichman ( 2014 ) in their meta‐analysis of displacement and diffusion effects of interventions in large‐scale geographic areas (see also Bowers, Johnson, Guerette, Summers, & Poynton,  2011 ; Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ). Effect sizes were calculated using the relative incidence risk ratio approach described above. Pre‐ and postintervention counts from the treatment buffer area(s) are compared with pre‐ and postintervention counts either from a control buffer area or to the control area itself in studies that did not have a catchment area for the control site. Figure  7 shows the mean overall effect for displacement/diffusion. Effects to the right of zero indicate evidence of diffusion of crime control benefits, while effects to the left suggest displacement effects.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g027.jpg

Displacement and diffusion‐combined effect size for study outcomes (a) Cohen's D (random effects model: Q  = 30.069, df  = 7, p  < .001, I 2  = 76.720) and (b) Log RIRR (random effects model: Q  = 30.547, df  = 7, p  < .001, I 2  = 77.085). CI, confidence interval

Looking at Cohen's D model (see Figure  7a ), the overall model provides no evidence of displacement. Seven of the eight studies have positive effect sizes, while the sole negative effect for Taylor et al. ( 2011 ) was very small and not statistically significant (−0.050, p  = .765). Moreover, the overall random effect for the model of 0.089 ( p  = .023) is suggestive of diffusion of benefits across these eight studies, though caution is needed given the small effect size and limited number of studies. We also estimated displacement/diffusion models using the largest and smallest effects to provide a range for our overall estimate. This showed that when using the smallest effects, there is no evidence of either displacement or diffusion (0.029, p  = .345), while when only including largest effects the evidence in favor of diffusion of benefits is moderately stronger than in the mean effects model (0.154, p  = .006). The results from the RIRR model lead to the same conclusions. The overall model (see Figure  7b ) shows a relative reduction of 17.5% ( p  = .005) in favor of diffusion effects and the largest effects model shows a larger relative decline of 34.6% ( p  = .004). The smallest effects model shows no statistical evidence of displacement or diffusion, with a nonsignificant relative difference of 4.6% ( p  = .327) in favor of diffusion.

5.5.3. Narrative review of impacts on noncrime/disorder outcomes

While the primary question of our review is concerned with crime and disorder outcomes of POP, we also collected data, when available, on the cost effectiveness of POP, as well as secondary outcomes of POP programs, including impacts on police legitimacy, fear of crime, and collective efficacy. Because only a small number of included studies focused on each of these outcomes and inconsistency in measures across studies, we felt a narrative review of findings would be more useful for synthesis than a meta‐analysis. Additionally, because our included studies generally prioritized crime control outcomes, the information provided on these secondary outcomes is not always sufficient for calculating effect sizes. We are also more cautious in interpreting these findings, since we did not search for, and excluded any studies we did find, that focused exclusively on fear of crime or other noncrime outcomes. A future study should systematically search for studies of POP focused on impacting outcomes other than crime and disorder.

We include summary information on each outcome below and an examination of findings by study in Table  7 . Our findings overall suggest POP can be cost‐effective, but tends to have limited impacts on police legitimacy, fear of crime, and collective efficacy, although those outcomes are often not assessed in our included studies. 13

Impacts of problem‐oriented policing noncrime/disorder outcomes

Financial cost‐benefit analysis

Eight studies assessed cost or hours savings as a result of the POP project. These were generally based on cost estimates for how much time would have been spent on calls for service or incidents that were prevented by the POP project. In most cases, estimates were just based on police time and cost, but two studies (Bichler et al.  2013 ; London Borough of Enfield,  2011 ) also included estimates for time saved by other agencies. In all cases, the POP project was associated with a substantial cost savings. We recognize though that POP projects without significant impacts on crime would be less likely to include a cost‐benefit analysis.

Two motel‐based studies in the U.S. looked just at hours saved. Bichler et al. ( 2013 ) examined just savings in hours, finding the motel ordinance program saved 1,253.4 hours per year in patrol time on calls for service, more than a 51% reduction. Time spent by other city agencies on motel‐related issues also dropped 92.4 hours per year, on average. The Reno Police Department ( 2006 ) did not provide precise estimates for their entire project, but noted that impacts had saved the department approximately 1,750 officer hours per year.

Two other U.S. studies estimated cost savings based on calls prevented in retail locations. White and Katz ( 2013 ) estimated the cost to respond to calls at targeted convenience stores dropped from $43,685 to $25,403. They argue these are conservative estimates, since they do not account for other criminal justice system and business costs. Zidar et al. ( 2017 ) found a program to reduce responses for low‐level theft led to $26,884 less in police department manpower costs. The department saved about 35 hours per month by responding to fewer calls at Walmart.

Four studies by U.K. agencies also estimated savings, using Home Office estimates on the costs of crime. Durham Constabulary ( 2017 ) found that burglaries prevented equated to a savings of £3,640 just in police‐related costs, with even higher estimates when accounting for all system and victim costs. Lancashire Constabulary ( 2008 ) estimated cost savings across multiple crime categories as a result of their POP project. Burglary savings were estimated at $62,100, criminal damage savings were $72,420, and antisocial behavior incident savings totaled $51,770. A second project by Lancashire Constabulary ( 2012 ) found a significant reduction in arrests were associated with a total cost savings of £82,000. The London Borough of Enfield ( 2011 ) estimated project cost savings at £934,000, accounting for both police and social costs of crimes prevented.

Police legitimacy/satisfaction

Six studies used measures of resident perceptions of police procedural justice and/or legitimacy to assess impacts of POP on trust in the police. Results here were not entirely consistent, but generally suggest POP has limited impact on police legitimacy. There is no evidence here, however, that problem‐oriented approaches, even when applied in hot spots, damage police trust.

Stone ( 1993 ) saw no change over time or across treatment and control public housing sites in Atlanta in whether residents were satisfied with police and thought police treated them with respect. Braga and Bond ( 2008 ) found no differences in perceptions of police in pre and post interviews with respondents who were likely to have had contact with police during the Lowell hot spots experiment. Cooley et al. ( 2019 ) similarly found no change over time or between treatment and control neighborhoods in perceptions of whether police are doing a good job. Results were similar in the two most rigorous assessments. Using a mail survey, Ratcliffe et al. ( 2015 ) found no differences in perceptions of procedural justice or satisfaction with police among residents of control spots versus those receiving POP in Philadelphia. Using an in‐person resident survey, Kochel and Weisburd ( 2017 ) found procedural justice perceptions improved over time in problem solving hot spots, but no more than they did in control hot spots. There was a small nonsignificant decline in problem solving hot spot resident perceptions of legitimacy in the short‐term follow‐up, but legitimacy had rebounded to similar levels to control respondents by the long‐term follow‐up. Tuffin et al. ( 2006 ) found the only evidence of enhanced perceptions of police, although we note that this was an evaluation of reassurance policing, so building trust was a major component along with problem solving. Here, treatment site residents relative to control site residents saw 15% net improvements in confidence in police and 8% net increases in feeling that police are willing to listen and respond.

Fear of crime

Eight studies assessed changes in resident fear of crime as a result of a POP project. Findings here were not entirely consistent, but most studies found no impact of POP on resident fear of crime, and for studies that did find an impact, effects were generally small. Stone's ( 1993 ) public housing surveys, Braga and Bond's ( 2009 ) resident interviews, Ratcliffe et al. ( 2015 ) mail surveys, and Cooley et al's. ( 2019 ) resident surveys, for example, suggested no pre–post change in fear in treatment relative to control sites. Tuffin et al. ( 2006 ) saw limited impacts of reassurance policing on fear of crime. There was some net improvement in feelings of safety after dark in treatment compared with control sites, but for particular crime types, there were no consistent effects. Stokes et al. ( 1996 ) saw, if anything, negative impacts of the safe route to school program on student fear of being attacked, not surprisingly given the overall backfire effects of the intervention. Similarly, Kochel et al. ( 2015 ) found increases in victimization risk and decreases in feelings of personal safety among residents of problem solving hot spots relative to controls in the short‐term, but there were no differences across groups by the second postintervention survey. Baker and Wolfer ( 2003 ) found more substantial impacts of the intervention on fear of crime among residents living near the target site, particularly for feelings of safety in the park during the day; however, even here the findings were mixed. Control group respondents were more likely than treatment group respondents to say they felt safe due to crime prevention efforts in the postsurvey, even though only treatment group residents had received a crime prevention intervention.

Collective efficacy

Three studies included pre‐ and postintervention measures of resident perceptions of collective efficacy. Findings across the three studies were inconsistent and mixed. None of the studies showed large impacts of POP on collective efficacy. Stone's ( 1993 ) survey showed no difference in perceptions of informal social control over time or in treatment versus control housing complexes. Tuffin et al. ( 2006 ) also found no difference in collective efficacy perceptions among residents of reassurance policing areas relative to control sites. There were also no significant changes in perceptions of social cohesion, but treatment sites did show improvements relative to comparison sites in the percentage of residents saying they trust many or some people in their area. Kochel and Weisburd ( 2019 ) found no overall change in resident perceptions of social cohesion or overall collective efficacy in problem‐solving hot spots relative to controls. They did find some long‐term improvements in perceptions of informal social control among POP hot spot residents, with about a 7% improvement compared with baseline levels. Kochel and Weisburd ( 2019 ) suggested the limited community involvement in most implemented problem‐solving projects may explain the modest impacts.

5.5.4. Publication bias

Publication bias presents a strong challenge to any review of evaluation studies (Rothstein,  2008 ). Campbell reviews, such as ours, take a number of steps to reduce publication bias, as represented by the fact that 21 of the 34 (61.8%) included studies in our main analyses came from unpublished sources (13 Goldstein Award Submissions, 4 research reports, 2 doctoral dissertations, and 2 Master's theses). Wilson has argued that there is often little difference in methodological quality between published and unpublished studies, suggesting the importance of searching the “gray literature” (Wilson,  2009 ). For our review, there may also be a bias in unpublished studies that are nevertheless available for review, since 13 studies were identified through the Goldstein Award competition. As noted earlier, award submissions are inherently biased toward successful programs. This was evidenced by our moderator analyses, which showed effect sizes were significantly larger for award submissions versus the other publication types.

Here we focus on an overall comparison of the 13 studies published in scholarly journals versus the other 21 studies (20 for the RIRR approach) through use of moderator analysis. For Cohen's D model, the mean overall effect size for studies published in scholarly journals is 0.156 ( p  = .002; Q  = 46.482, df  = 12, p  < .001) and for unpublished studies the average effect is 0.199 ( p  < .001; Q  = 111.367, df  = 20, p  < .001). Moreover, the moderated effect size is 0.184 ( p  < .001) and the between models heterogeneity test was not statistically significant ( Q  = 0.468, df  = 1, p  = .494). Similar findings are seen in the RIRR model. The mean effect for the scholarly journal studies shows a relative decline of 29.7% ( p  = .005; Q  = 63.518, df  = 12, p  < .001) while the unpublished studies show a relative decline of 35.1% ( p  < .001; Q  = 141.113, df  = 19, p  < .001). The moderated effect size shows a relative decline of 33.8% and the between models differences test was again not statistically significant ( Q  = 0.145, df  = 1, p  = .703). The lack of significance for the between‐model tests, along with the similarity between the mean overall effect sizes, suggests that publication bias may not have major impact on the outcomes of this review.

To more formally assess publication bias we generated a funnel plot to examine for possible selection bias in our results. This is shown in Figure  8a , ​ ,b. b . A visual inspection indicates some asymmetry with more studies with a large effect and a large standard error to the right of the mean than the left of the mean. We used the trim‐and‐fill procedure developed by Duval and Tweedie ( 2000 ) to examine how our estimates would change in the absence of this asymmetry. The trim‐and‐fill procedure determined that nine studies should be added to create symmetry.

Funnel plot to assess for publication bias. (a) Cohen's D . Empty circles are the studies included in our analyses, while the filled in circles indicate nine imputed studies for the trim‐and‐fill analysis. These additional studies lowered changed the mean effect size from 0.183 (95% CI = 0.0124–0.241) to 0.106 (95% CI = 0.043‐0.170). (b) Log RIRR. Empty circles are the studies included in our analyses, while the filled in circles indicate 11 imputed studies for the trim‐and‐fill analysis. These additional studies lowered the mean effect size from 0.291 (95% CI = 0.202–0.379) to 0.132 (95% CI = 0.040–0.223). In terms of relative risk reduced in treatment versus control groups this represents a decrease from the observed effect from 33.8% in the observed data to 14.1%. CI, confidence interval

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g017.jpg

For Cohen's D model, these additional nine imputed studies slightly reduced the mean effect size from 0.183 (95% CI = 0.0124–0.241) to 0.106 (95% CI = 0.043–0.170). For the RIRR approach, 11 studies were imputed and this reduced the overall effect from 0.291 (95% CI = 0.202–0.379) to 0.132 (95% CI = 0.040–0.223). Put into relative risk reduction terms, the imputed studies decreased the overall relative reduction from 33.8% to 14.1%.

Along with the moderator analysis above, this suggests that while there is some potential for publication bias in our sample, especially given the nature of including police award submissions, it does not alter our overall conclusion of POP having an overall significant, meaningful impact on crime and disorder.

6. DISCUSSION

6.1. summary of main results.

The results of this updated review provide strong evidence that POP is effective in reducing crime. Across a large array of analyses, we find statistically significant impacts of POP. Overall we find that there was a 33.8% reduction in crime/disorder in the POP treatment areas/groups relative to the controls. At the same time, the effect sizes we observe are strongly heterogeneous and the overall effect is likely overstated as smaller effects were found when looking only at the randomized experimental evaluations and after accounting for publication bias. Nonetheless, the findings of those models still show that POP is associated with meaningful and statistically significant declines in crime/disorder in treatment groups relative to controls. Such heterogeneity across models is very common in meta‐analyses in criminology (Lösel,  2018 ), but points to the importance of going beyond what works in POP to what are the most effective strategies for specific problems.

Overall, our findings show that police following the tenets of the SARA model to identify specific problems, conduct analyses to examine underlying causes, and develop and deliver tailor‐made responses is an evidence‐based approach to crime prevention. This is especially true for property crime and disorder offenses based on our results. POP is also an approach that fits well with hot spots policing, another tactic that has been found effective in reducing crime through Campbell reviews (Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ). Some of our studies overlap with those in that review as they involve applying problem‐solving tactics at small hot spots of crime/disorder, and the authors of the hot spots review note that their strongest effects were associated with programs that involved problem‐solving efforts rather than just increased police presence/activity. The positive findings of both reviews suggest that combining the two approaches is likely a fruitful endeavor for crime‐control efforts.

As a number studies involved place‐based POP programs, it was important to also examine the potential for spatial displacement and diffusion of crime control benefits. Eight of our 26 place‐based studies reported data that allowed us to calculate effect sizes for displacement/diffusion. There is no evidence of spatial displacement of crime/disorder in these studies. Indeed, there is evidence of a small diffusion effect when looking at the mean effect across outcomes. This finding, along with a similar finding of a small diffusion effect in the hot spots policing review (Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ), suggests that place‐based policing efforts do not simply cause crime to “move around the corner” (see Weisburd et al.,  2006 ).

There was some evidence that research design moderated the magnitude of the impact of POP on crime/disorder. The effect size for quasi‐experiments was somewhat larger than that for randomized experiments. However, the mean overall effect is significant for both models (as well as the moderated effect size). The same was true when examining the award submissions, which are inherently biased toward success and larger effects, as those had larger effects than the other types of studies. Nonetheless the overall effect for the non‐award submission studies was statistically significant. Adding to this the fact that nearly all of the studies were weighted to the prevention side of the distribution across analyses, we have additional confidence in our overall conclusion.

Finally, as noted above, if underlying causes at problem places are successfully addressed, the crime‐reduction benefits at targeted locations may be longer lasting and more meaningful in terms of overall crimes prevented compared with a similar effect size for a temporary police crackdown on an area or an intervention delivered to individual offenders. In plain language, there may be more “bang for our buck” when lasting changes are made at places, versus crackdowns that see deterrent effects erode when the program ends or person‐based programs that have to be continually delivered to different individuals over time. Unfortunately, existing studies do not examine crime prevention benefits in the long run, and are generally limited to follow‐up within a year or less (see also Weisburd & Majmundar,  2018 for a similar critique of short‐term follow‐up periods for POP studies). Future evaluations should include longer follow‐up periods so that later updates to this review can quantitatively assess the potential lasting impacts of POP.

6.2. Overall completeness and applicability of evidence

The findings of this review have widespread applicability to policing and crime prevention given the consistency of the conclusions drawn across all of our models. This review also represents a large increase in the available body of evidence in the time since the original review, which included only 10 studies (4 randomized experiments and 6 quasi‐experiments) and 16 outcomes. The current review includes an additional 24 studies (5 randomized experiments and 19 quasi‐experiments) published through December 2018. Our overall model now provides a meta‐analysis on the impact of POP on a total of 70 crime and disorder outcomes across 34 studies. The inclusion of these additional studies reaffirms the conclusion of the original review about POP's effectiveness in reducing crime and disorder with support from a much larger number of tests. While most studies (82.4%) were conducted in the United States, the fact that 5 were conducted in the United Kingdom and 1 in Canada offer initial support that POP is applicable in different contexts. However, this is still limited and caution may be needed when trying to generalize these findings to contexts outside of North America and the United Kingdom. We also were unable to perform a meaningful meta‐analysis on noncrime outcomes. With the current data we could only provide a preliminary, narrative summary of study findings due to the small number of studies that report on the same noncrime outcomes captured through similar measures. The same was true for cost‐benefit analysis.

6.3. Quality of evidence

The overall quality of evidence has improved from the original review with the inclusion of 5 new randomized experiments and 19 quasi‐experiments. However, as we discussed above and assessed with our moderator analyses, POP remains an area that needs more rigorous evaluations. The majority of studies are still quasi‐experiments, and several are weaker designs with unmatched control groups, comparisons of a target area to the rest of a jurisdiction and so forth. While we have confidence in our conclusions as the main finding of a significant effect for POP holds when looking only at the most rigorous studies, caution is needed in individually interpreting the larger effects from the weaker studies—especially the award submissions which are inherently biased toward positive outcomes.

We note that while more randomized experiments, and quasi‐experiments with matched control groups/areas, would improve the quality of evidence on POP, the existing body of evidence is largely a function of the nature of the POP model. The approach calls for identifying specific problems to be researched and addressed and it is often not going to be possible to create a well‐matched comparison area in a study jurisdiction (Eck,  2006a ,  2006b ). Similarly, the POP model calls on police to identify, analyze and respond to problems, and to then assess whether their efforts are successful. In this sense, the award submissions are evidence of the SARA model in action and are important to include in reviews such as this.

Moreover, the fact that 11 additional award submissions were eligible for this updated review (only two were included in the meta‐analysis in the original review) is indicative of an increase in rigor as more agencies are using control groups, even if unmatched, instead of simple pre–post case study designs. As such, these studies are important evidence and simply require caution in interpreting individual effect sizes and acknowledgment that there is a “file drawer” problem here as agencies are not going to submit (or even write up research reports) for programs that failed. On that front, we retain confidence in our findings as our analyses above suggest that publication bias was not a major concern in our study.

6.4. Limitations and potential biases in the review process

In general, there are no specific limitations or biases in the review process used in this study beyond those inherent to the systematic review process. Namely, all reviews will have a “file drawer” problem to some extent, and the threat may be slightly higher for POP than other approaches due to the assessment step of the SARA model asking police to test whether their efforts were effective. As discussed above, these findings are unlikely to be written up (much less submitted for award consideration) when efforts fail. Moreover, the use of the Global Policing Database is a huge asset to the current review. The traditional and gray literature sources searched to compile that database are far more exhaustive than those used in individual reviews (see Appendices B and C ), including the original version of this review. Lastly, there were 15 (13 of which were award submissions) potentially eligible studies of POP that we could not include as we could not calculate standardized effect sizes due to insufficient or inadequate information being presented (see Appendix F ). As noted above, we do not feel the absence of these studies biased our conclusions as the 13 award submissions discussed positive impacts of POP and the other two studies reported null effects, but no backfire effects.

6.5. Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews

The findings of this review reaffirm those of the earlier review (Weisburd et al.,  2008 ,  2010 ) and existing narrative reviews that conclude that current evidence suggests that POP is one of the most promising police approaches to preventing crime (Skogan & Frydl,  2004 ; Weisburd and Majmundar,  2018 ). Moreover, the limited evidence on displacement and diffusion confirms the findings of other studies of place‐based crime prevention efforts by showing evidence of diffusion of benefits (Bowers et al.,  2011 ; Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ; Weisburd & Majmundar,  2018 ; Weisburd et al.,  2006 ). This finding is contrary to arguments made by others that displacement is the likely outcome of place‐based interventions (Blattman, Green, Ortega, & Tobón,  2017 ; Reppetto,  1976 ).

7. AUTHORS’ CONCLUSIONS

7.1. implications for practice and policy.

Evidence from this review suggests that POP is an effective crime prevention approach. While there is a great deal of heterogeneity in effect sizes across studies and outcomes, 31 out of 34 studies (91.2%) have effect sizes in favor of a treatment effect. Moreover, the overall effect is positive and significant in all of our mean effect size models. In short, the findings suggest that POP is a promising approach to reducing a variety of types of crime and disorder in a variety of contexts (the 34 included studies included 29 unique jurisdictions in 3 countries).

Our findings suggest that it is important for police departments to be fully behind POP efforts if they are to succeed. For instance, the lone backfire effect in the study (Stokes et al.,  1996 ) involved a program that was barely implemented as two‐thirds of students at the target school were unaware of the existence of the school safety corridor and the corridor was poorly staffed in the after school hours due to the timing of police shift changes and limited police resources. Similarly, a null effect was reported in the study by Stone ( 1993 ), who noted that the police department did not seem entirely interested in implementing POP and that study officers did not view problem solving as “real” police work. This and other factors led to the program being chronically understaffed.

However, our results also highlight the fact that POP efforts can be successful even if the SARA approach is not delivered in its ideal version. This is important as studies have noted that it is difficult to fully implement the ideal model (Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ; Weisburd & Braga, 2006 ; Cordner & Biebel,  2005 ; Eck,  2006b ; Maguire et al.,  2015 ). Our findings show that even though the SARA model is often loosely followed, with the problem analysis often being shallow rather than in‐depth (see Table  2b ), the approach is still found to lead to crime reductions when compared with control areas that received standard police services. This adds support to arguments that even “shallow” problem‐solving efforts can be lead to significant reductions in crime (Braga & Weisburd,  2010 ).

POP can also be fruitfully combined with other police tactics that have been found effective in recent Campbell reviews. In particularly, the POP approach has been shown to be effective when combined with hot spots policing. Braga, Weisburd, et al. ( 2019 ) found larger effect sizes for POP approaches at hot spots than approaches which simply increased police presence/activity at target areas. Elements of POP often also underlie the focused deterrence approach, which has been found effective and could perhaps be enhanced with more in‐depth problem solving in future programs rather than largely replicating the existing “pulling levers” model (Braga, Weisburd, et al.,  2019 ).

There is also potential for combining POP with the popular Compstat model. Indeed, “innovative problem solving” is one of the key elements in the ideal form of Compstat. While evaluations of Compstat in the United States suggest that the emphasis on holding commanders accountable through Compstat meetings has limited innovative problem solving in the field (Weisburd, Willis, Mastrofski, & Greenspan,  2019 ), a recent Israeli program suggests that innovative problem solving can be encouraged in a Compstat‐like reform (see Weisburd et al., Unpublished manuscript). In that study, evidence‐based policing practices were strongly encouraged in the context of a national POP reform program, and the message of the program was communicated more directly to the rank and file. Robust prevention benefits were identified in moderate and large size police agencies in quasi‐experimental analyses of property crimes.

7.2. Implications for research

Our study identified 70 tests of POP in 34 included studies. Our meta‐analyses suggest that overall there is a significant effect of the approach in reducing crime and disorder. Our moderator analyses showed that effects are larger for the quasi‐experiments compared with the randomized experiments. Nonetheless, this does highlight the need for more rigorous evaluations of POP in order for a future update of this review to provide a more robust estimate of overall effect size.

This is not in any way meant to discourage quasi‐experimental evaluations of POP, or even pre–post case studies. As discussed earlier, the nature of the POP model means there may sometimes only be a single area with the problem being treated, and even if there are multiple locations it will often be difficult, and sometimes impossible, to identify suitable control areas—much less to identify enough sites to create a randomized experiment with sufficient statistical power. As such, knowledge from less rigorous research remains important. That said, given the similarity of our current findings to those of our original review, we view it as unlikely that future updates will shed new light on our knowledge of POP's effectiveness in the absence of more rigorous evaluations.

In our view, the question at this point is not whether POP works. The vast majority of the studies we reviewed show prevention benefits. This conclusion is also supported by past narrative reviews (Skogan & Frydl,  2004 ; Weisburd & Majmundar,  2018 ) and the summary of simple pre–post case studies provided in our original review (Weisburd et al.,  2008 ,  2010 ). The key remaining questions surround what characteristics are associated with larger impacts of POP on crime. To truly assess this, future evaluations need to not only be more rigorous, but also must capture and report more data about the problems targeted, the level of problem analysis applied, the specific responses actually delivered and report outcomes more often by crime type than aggregate categories. The current sample of studies have a combination of lack of detailed information on many of these factors and tremendous heterogeneity in what is reported, which makes it difficult to draw strong conclusions about what makes POP most effective.

But irrespective of reporting practices, to build an evidence base that will be useful for practitioners in the field it is important for there to be a robust evidence base that is related to specific problems and specific interventions. This is a limitation more generally in the crime and justice field, but is particularly important to address when we have strong evidence of crime prevention effectiveness of a strategy, as we do here. Practitioners want to know what works in what situations, and which practices are most cost‐effective. Building such an evidence base would take a major federal or foundation effort to advance the practice of POP, and literally hundreds of studies testing practices in regard to specific types of problems. In turn, we have limited cost‐benefit analysis data from the studies we reviewed. The studies that did examine cost savings generally used limited data to estimate both costs and benefits and rarely did any systematic analysis. For practice it is not simply whether something works, it is equally important to provide a sense of what cost for what benefit. Answering this question should be a major focus of future studies.

The authors of this review plan to do a deeper dive and coding of the eligible studies to see if more light can be shed on these matters in a follow‐up publication. The prospects of succeeding in this effort with existing data are unclear, and was beyond the scope and timeline for this funded review.

On this front, it is important for more future studies to evaluate the impact of POP on outcomes beyond the standard crime and disorder outcomes examined in the studies included in this review. These studies tested impacts on aggregate crime/disorder, violence, property crime, disorder, drug sales/use and related outcomes such as probation/parole success or failure. POP was proposed as a flexible model that can be applied to a wide array of problems and our understanding of the model's potential would be enhanced through studies that assess its impact on issues such as cyber‐crime, human trafficking and other issues increasingly of interest to criminologists and criminal justice practitioners. Additionally, more future studies should be designed to examine its applicability in reducing resident fear of crime, improving citizens’ opinions of the police, and bolstering collective efficacy. Too few existing evaluations report on such outcomes to allow for a meaningful meta‐analysis, but our narrative review of the existing evidence suggests mixed and inconsistent findings across studies. This suggests the need for further research, particularly on POP interventions that include close partnership with and involvement of the community, which might be expected to have the greatest impacts on these perception‐based outcomes.

Lastly, this updated review also added the approach of performing meta‐analyses using log RIRRs as the effect sizes. This was done based upon in‐progress work by David Wilson which argues both that Cohen's D fails to produce effect sizes that are comparable across studies when based on place‐based count data and that the conversion of RIRR to Cohen's D is problematic. We still reported Cohen's D , including a majority (27 out of 34) of effects that were converted from RIRR, as we wanted to be consistent with the approach used in our original review (Weisburd et al., 2008 ,  2010 ) and other recent Campbell Reviews such as the updated hot spots policing review (Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ). This also allowed us to compare the two approaches.

Our results show that while similar conclusions would be reached about POP's effectiveness using either approach, it does appear based on our comparisons and examples discussed above in Section  5.5 that the Cohen's D approach may understate the impact of place‐based interventions, and that the RIRR approach appears to generate effect sizes that are more in line with actual changes reported in the studies themselves. Moreover, being able to convert the log RIRR to relative change in the treatment group versus the control group makes effect sizes more intuitive for researchers and practitioners alike. For instance, for our Cohen's D model our mean overall effect of 0.183 is not going to immediately tell a police leader much about the effectiveness of POP. However, using the RIRR approach allows us to more simply state that the relative reduction in the POP groups versus the controls was 33.8%. This is much more easily interpretable to practitioners, which is an important aim of Campbell Systematic Reviews.

Given that the RIRR approach is both more informative for practitioners and, based upon David Wilson's ( in progress ) work, more appropriate for place‐based studies (while also avoiding the problematic conversion of RIRR to Cohen's D ) we encourage future meta‐analyses of place‐based interventions to adopt this method.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

Weisburd is an author on four of the included studies and has been author or coauthor of several studies that have found POP and other proactive policing approaches effective, coauthored a book on POP with Anthony Braga and served on National Academy of Science panels which concluded that POP is a promising approach for crime prevention. Telep has coauthored a problem‐solving guide for the POP Center and has coauthored narrative reviews of policing strategies and helped design the Evidence‐Based Policing Matrix which suggests POP and similar approaches are effective based on existing evidence. Weisburd and Telep do not have any ideological bias toward the effectiveness of POP. Nonetheless, the inclusion of additional authors without prior work in this area reduces unconscious biases. Hinkle and Petersen have not conducted evaluation research or published on the effectiveness of POP outside of this Campbell Review (including the original version for Hinkle).

ROLES AND RESPONSIBILITES

J. C. H., D. W., and C. W. T. designed the original systematic review following established Campbell Collaboration conventions and procedures, with assistance from John Eck and Phyllis Shultz. J. C. H., D. W., and C. W. T., designed the updated review with assistance from the GPD team of Elizabeth Eggins, Lorraine Mazerolle, Angela Higginson. This team also performed the search of the GPD and sent results to J. C. H. Forward searches of seminal POP studies and manual inspection of recent volumes of leading journals and the submissions to Goldstein and Tilley awards were performed by K. P. Title and abstract screening for eligible studies was performed by K. P., with any studies that were not obviously eligible or ineligible reviewed and ruled upon via a vote by J. C. H., D. W., and C. W. T. Coding of each study was performed by K. P. and one other graduate student assistant (either Julia Durska or Taryn Zastrow). All discrepancies between the two coders were reviewed, discussed and resolved via vote by J. C. H., D. W., and C. W. T. All effect sizes were calculated by J. C. H. (with some help from David Wilson and Anthony Braga). All analyses were conducted by J. C. H. The literature review and methodology sections of the report were written by J. C., H. D. W., and C. W. T. Summary information about studies (narrative reviews, review of reported implementation problems, review of reported bias and the associated tables for these sections) was drafted by K. P. The narrative review of impacts on noncrime outcomes was written by C. W. T. The results and discussion/conclusion sections were written in close collaboration between J. C. H., D. W. and C. W. T. All authors read, edited and commented on all sections of the report.

  • Content: J. C. H., D. W., C. W. T., and K. P.
  • Systematic review methods: J. C. H., D. W., C. W. T.
  • Statistical analysis: J. C. H.
  • Information retrieval: Elizabeth Eggins, Lorraine Mazerolle, Angela Higginson, J. C. H., and K. P.

SOURCES OF SUPPORT

This updated review was supported by the Campbell Collaboration through funding provided by Problem Solving and Demand Reduction Programme, hosted by the South Yorkshire Police. The original review was supported by Award 2007‐IJ‐CX‐0045, awarded by the National Institute of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, U.S. Department of Justice and the Nordic Campbell Centre.

PLANS FOR UPDATING THE REVIEW

Joshua Hinkle will coordinate the next update of this review with support from David Weisburd, Cody Telep and Kevin Petersen. We plan to update this review every 5 years, in accordance with Campbell Collaboration Guidelines. As the search strategy of this review depends upon the GPD, the plan is to carry out an update when five additional full years of studies (work through December 2023) have been fully indexed into the database.

APPENDIX A. NARRATIVE SUMMARY OF INCLUDED STUDIES

Baker and wolfer ( 2003 ).

Baker and Wolfer ( 2003 ) describe a problem‐oriented policing (POP) intervention in a small Pennsylvania town aimed at targeting vandalism and substance use in a local park. During the scanning and analysis process, officers noted that the park was full of litter and had overgrown brush, allowing offenders to hide from police. Using crime prevention surveys and crime mapping, they determined that the problem was isolated in the small area in and around the park. To respond, officers target hardened by removing overgrown shrubs. They used other methods of situational crime prevention by installing cameras, repairing fences, improving lighting, locking the park at night, limiting access, and posting rules and regulations. In addition, the police used proactive patrol and increased enforcement of the curfew law to target juvenile offenders. Officers worked with residents to establish a Neighborhood Watch to coordinate cooperation between the police and area residents. To assess the project, researchers used a quasi‐experimental design with a comparison group. Volunteers administered 29‐question surveys both before and after the project to random samples of residents in the immediate area of the park and a comparison group of residents who lived in the same town, but not adjacent to the park. Pre‐assessment survey results indicated that residents in the target group reported witnessing significantly more vandalism and public drinking/disorder than residents in the comparison group. However, target group residents reported a greater reduction in these behaviors by the post‐assessment period, and the difference between the groups was no longer statistically significant. Target group residents also reported a greater reduction in overall victimization and larger increases in feelings of safety. Some evidence of displacement and dispersion was reported, but no tests of statistical significance were presented.

Bichler et al. ( 2013 ); Chula Vista Police Department ( 2009 )

The City of Chula Vista's tourist economy had been suffering as a result of high crime and disorder at budget motels. Due to the economic ramifications, business organizations began to reach out to the police and local government in hopes of partnering to address the problem. Initial efforts to address motel crime through increased enforcement were deemed unsuccessful, leading to an alternative problem‐solving approach. The police began analyzing trends in calls for service reports and call narratives, surveying motel users, interviewing managers, and conducting environmental assessments. Analysis indicated that management policies and practices were facilitating crime and that place managers may be an effective target for the response. The proceeding intervention unfolded in three separate stages. In the outreach stage, the project staff began producing calls for service report cards for each motel and distributing them individually to motel operators. These reports summarized the level of crime at each respective motel in an effort to increase awareness on the part of motel operators. The outreach stage also included seminars held for motel managers to provide technical assistance as well as education on safety improvements. Following outreach, project staff began the code enforcement stage. With cooperation from local government, motel facilities were inspected, and violations were enforced. Additionally, staff began distributing motel calls for service rankings to all managers in attempt to shame them into making changes. When the problem persisted, a permit ordinance stage was initiated. In this stage an annual permit to operate, based on calls for service rates, was instituted via a multi‐agency collaborative effort. Motels with call levels under a certain threshold were granted license, while those above the threshold were required to enter into a memorandum of understanding that required appropriate action to address crime problems. To assess the impact of the intervention, Bichler et al. ( 2013 ) employed a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. To search for valid comparison units, researchers identified motels within the same region that experienced similar levels of crime and environmental characteristics (such as proximity to major freeways). This process led to the selection of 10 motels from a nearby city. Calls for service rates at these comparison motels were then compared with the calls for service rates at 15 intervention motels during both pre‐ and postintervention time periods. Additionally, researchers selected nine displacement/diffusion motels within proximal distance to the target motels. Raw count comparisons revealed a greater overall reduction in calls for service at intervention motels compared with control motels, and χ 2 analysis indicated that the overall change was statistically significant. Further, there was no evidence of spatial displacement in the displacement/diffusion motels. The evaluation also examined police and city costs, as measured by personnel hours devoted to the problem. Results indicated notable reductions in dedicated hours across all categories in the postintervention period, suggesting that the program was cost‐effective.

Bond and Hajjar ( 2013 )

As part of a Smart Policing Initiative (SPI), the Lowell (MA) Police Department (LPD) partnered with local researchers to implement a problem‐solving intervention targeting property crime hot spots. Using incident data derived from the LPD's crime analysis unit, the SPI team discussed the location of potential hot spots and ultimately selected four hot spots in each of the city's three police sectors. Comparison hot spots were then chosen based on similarities to target areas in both crime and other characteristics, resulting in a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design with four matched pairs. As part of the analysis process, captains from each sector completed biweekly surveys documenting the strategies being used in each hot spot. At these biweekly meetings staff reviewed responses and analyzed outcome data, resulting in an iterative problem‐solving process. While specific problem dynamics varied by hot spot, all areas focused on various forms of property crime (i.e., larceny, theft from a motor‐vehicle, burglary, shoplifting) as well as drug violations and prostitution. Response activities generally included directed patrol, traffic enforcement, increased visibility, community meetings, and in some cases partnership with inspectional and neighborhood services. To assess the effectiveness of the intervention, property crime incident counts for each hot spot were aggregated at the sector level. Raw incident counts were then compared before and after the intervention for both treatment and control groups. Simple comparison of percentages indicated that treatment hot spots witnessed a greater decline in aggregate property crime within each sector than comparison hot spots, with reductions ranging from 16% to 19% per sector. Additionally, anecdotal evidence suggested that the problem‐solving initiative may have improved communication and organizational functioning within the LPD.

Boston Police Department ( 2008 )

The Boston Police Department ( 2008 ) noticed increasing public concerns at neighborhood and community meetings over home security and residential burglary rates. An analysis of official data and incident maps suggested an increasing concentration of residential burglary incidents in District D‐14. These data also revealed several consistently problematic days within the week. Police began conducting environmental assessments of addresses experiencing three or more residential burglaries within the past year, revealing numerous security weaknesses. Reviews of prior call handling also revealed a lack of attention to detail in these investigations, and conversations with tenants and property management highlighted security risks in current management practices. Based on these data sources, the police developed a three‐pronged response plan. First, the department increased resources devoted to burglary investigations by assigning dedicated detectives to incidents and partnering with the city's housing division to make security changes to residential buildings. Second, the department held meetings with building owners and residents to educate them about target hardening techniques and general safety measures. Finally, police focused on hot spot enforcement by increasing presence and surveillance near areas that experienced the most burglary incidents. This included an undercover operation in collegiate residential areas during the spring break period. The intervention was assessed using a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. The evaluation compared residential burglary incident counts for the year preceding the intervention to the year after implementation. Incident counts were compared between District D‐14 and the rest of the city. The assessment showed promising results, with the intervention district accounting for only 12.4% of the citywide residential burglaries in the postintervention year, as compared with 20.5% in the year preceding. The authors also noted that there was no evidence of displacement, based on measurement of burglary rates in nearby jurisdictions. However, no statistical tests of displacement were presented.

Braga et al. ( 1999 ); Braga ( 1997 )

Braga and colleagues (1999) document a POP project in Jersey City, NJ designed to address hot spots of violent crime. These hot spots were defined using computerized mapping and then officers worked to determine what problems existed at each hot spot. After initially choosing 28 pairs of violent crime places, the randomized experiment was narrowed to 12 pairs: 12 hot spots received POP and 12 received traditional patrol. Additionally, two‐block catchment areas were built around each of the 24 places to test for displacement effects. In the 12 treatment pairs, officers were required to complete an analysis report assessing the specific problems in the particular hot spot. They were encouraged to use official data and meetings with, or surveys of, community members. Although all the hot spots were chosen because of high rates of violence (typically street fights, drug market violence, and/or robbery), officers also identified widespread disorder problems that included public drinking and loitering. Officers designed a response to specifically target the problems they uncovered in the analysis stage. Thus, the exact response varied by hot spot, but the responses all included some aspect of aggressive order maintenance and most included efforts to make physical improvements to the area (e.g., removing trash, improving lighting) and drug enforcement. To assess the project, the researchers used calls for service data, incident data, and pre‐ and postobservations of physical and social disorder. Crime incident and calls for service outcomes were assessed using a 6‐month preintervention and 6‐month postintervention time period. Generalized linear models indicated a main effect of treatment on both aggregate criminal incidents and calls for service, as treatment places were associated with significant reductions in both measures compared with control locations. Further analysis revealed significant reductions in street fighting, property, and narcotics calls for service, as well as robbery and property incidents at treatment places relative to control. Pre‐ and postobservations of social and physical disorder indicated that, in both categories, disorder was reduced at 10 of 11 treatment locations (one location was dropped from analysis), and these reductions also reached statistical significance. Displacement/diffusion results did reveal significant displacement of property crime incidents to catchment areas. However, no other crime types showed evidence of displacement, and several outcomes may have shown evidence of diffusion of benefits.

Braga and Bond ( 2008 ,  2009 )

Braga and Bond ( 2008 ) evaluated a research‐practitioner partnership with the Lowell, MA Police Department (LPD) to implement a problem‐solving initiative targeting hot spots of overall crime and disorder. As part of the scanning phase, crime and disorder‐related calls for service were mapped for the prior year, and temporal analyses were conducted to identify areas with persistently high call levels. Hot spot boundaries were then identified with input from LPD officers and consideration of place characteristics, resulting in 34 distinct hot spots. For each hot spot, a two‐block catchment area was also constructed to measure potential displacement and diffusion of benefits. Following a randomized experimental design, the 34 hot spots were matched into 17 pairs (based on the qualitative characteristics of the areas) and then randomly assigned to treatment or control from within each pair. Treatment hot spots were assigned to police captains who were then required to submit analysis reports detailing the causes of the area's crime problems and the proposed response measures. Analysis sources for treatment hot spots generally consisted of official data and conversations with community members. Throughout the intervention the LPD held monthly meetings with captains at which current strategies would be assessed and amended if appearing to be unsuccessful. Specific responses varied by hot spot, but all measures were broadly considered either situational strategies (cleaning vacant lots, nuisance abatement, improved lighting and video surveillance, code inspections, etc.), social service strategies (providing mental health resources to problem tenants, working with shelters for the homeless, providing recreational activities for youth, etc.), or order maintenance strategies (increased enforcement of disorder offenses, stop and frisk usage, targeting drug dealers, directed patrol, etc.). The control hot spots received standard policing. To assess the results of the experiment, calls for service rates for treatment and control hot spots were compared pre‐ and postintervention across a number of crime and disorder categories (assault, robbery, breaking and entering, larceny/theft, disorder/nuisance). In addition, pre–post observations of both physical and social disorder were conducted for treatment and control places. Analysis of calls for services outcomes was conducted using a 6‐month preintervention and 6‐month postintervention time period. Poisson and negative binomial regression models indicated significant reductions in assault, robbery, burglary, and disorder/nuisance calls for service in treatment hot spots relative to control hot spots. There were no significant changes in larceny/theft, however. These results also translated into a significant overall reduction in calls for service at treatment locations compared with control areas. Observations of physical and social disorder further revealed significant decreases in both categories for treatment locations compared with controls (physical disorder was reduced in 14 of 17 hot spots while social disorder was reduced in 13 of 17). In addition, there was no statistically significant evidence of either displacement or diffusion of benefits. Researchers also conducted a mediation analysis including the three main program elements (misdemeanor arrests, situational strategies, and social service strategies) as mediators between treatment and calls for service. Results of this analysis indicated that situational strategies produced the most significant crime reduction effect.

Cooley et al. ( 2019 )

The Canton (OH) Police Department (CPD) initiated a POP program targeting disorderly conditions in the Homestead neighborhood. Informal surveys with neighborhood residents indicated that there were concerns regarding quality of life issues. Officers were instructed to go into the neighborhood and take note of problems they would want to change if they lived in the area. Officers stayed in the neighborhood for extended periods of time. They learned that a frequent complaint from residents was that of landlords who facilitated crime by housing problem tenants. In response, the police created a database of problem landlords and confronted them directly. The CPD also attempted to increase trust in police by working with neighborhood groups and making appearances at a job training center for at‐risk youth. Other response activities included focused deterrence with repeat offenders, and partnership with parole to assist with offender reintegration. The evaluation conducted by Cooley et al. ( 2019 ) is an attempted replication of the Homestead intervention. The replication was conducted in another Canton, Ohio neighborhood that was also experiencing crime and disorder problems (McCormick neighborhood). The evaluation used a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Counts of both violent offenses and quality of life offenses were compared between the treatment neighborhood and a comparison neighborhood (selected based on similar crime problems and neighborhood characteristics) across 12‐month pre‐ and postintervention time periods. Results indicated decreases in both violent and quality of life offenses for the treatment area, compared with minor increases in both categories for the control area. However, difference‐in‐difference analyses indicated that the magnitude of decline was not large enough in either category to reach the conventional level of statistical significance. Additionally, there were no significant differences found between the two groups in satisfaction with police or fear of crime.

Dario ( 2016 ); Glendale Police Department ( 2016 ); White and Katz ( 2013 )

In 2009 researchers from Arizona State University's Center for Violence Prevention and Community Safety partnered with the Glendale (AZ) Police Department on a SPI to target property crime using a problem‐oriented approach. As part of the SPI, officers were trained on the SARA model and were instructed to identify persistent problems. Officers selected convenience store crime due to its persistence and consequences for the department and community. During analysis, the team analyzed calls for service data for all 65 Glendale convenience stores and determined that the top 10 call generators were all Circle K stores. The stores were then mapped to determine if these trends were the result of location. Mapping revealed significant differences in call levels, even between Circle K stores and other convenience stores in the same area. Environmental surveys and CPTED assessments of Circle K stores were conducted, and the team noted numerous security issues in store design and management practice. To respond to these issues the SPI team developed a three‐pronged response that targeted the top six call generating Circle Ks. In response I, the SPI team met with Circle K leadership and presented the results of their CPTED assessments and recommendations for change. While some changes were implemented, this stage was met with limited success. The SPI team then convened other law‐enforcement agencies in the area, compared calls for service data, and determined that Circle K stores were problematic in other areas as well. This report was then provided to local media, resulting in coverage of the crime problem as a public shaming mechanism. In response II, the team developed prevention messages with help from local government that advertised the risks of convenience store theft. These messages were targeted at middle school and high school students. Finally, in response III the team engaged in suppression efforts involving increased surveillance and enforcement. To evaluate the intervention, White & Katz ( 2013 ) used a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. They compared call rates at the top six Circle K stores to call rates for the remaining nine Circle K stores in the city, as well as the top 13 other Glendale convenience stores. Statistical comparisons were conducted for the year prior to and year after the intervention. Using an analysis of variance model, White and Katz found a significant overall decrease of 42% in calls for service at the six intervention Circle K stores, with five of six stores experiencing individually significant call decreases. The nine comparison Circle K stores experienced an overall decrease of 31% in calls for service, however, this change was not statistically significant. Further, the non‐Circle K stores used for comparison experienced an overall increase of 0.5% from pre‐ to postintervention. Dario ( 2016 ) subsequently extended the pre‐ and postintervention periods by nearly a year and a half to assess the longitudinal effects of the program, while also including additional comparison stores. Difference‐in‐difference estimation and negative binomial regression further indicated a statistically significant effect of treatment. Intervention stores were found to have experienced 16.47% fewer calls for service relative to comparison stores, though the regression results were only significant for the full model (intervention stores vs. all nonintervention stores, rather than only nonintervention Circle K stores). Dario also conducted a displacement/diffusion analysis, finding no evidence of spatial displacement, but rather finding statistically significant evidence of a diffusion of benefits for five of six intervention stores. However, Dario noted mixed evidence of crime‐type displacement varying by individual store.

Durham Constabulary ( 2017 )

Police managers and researchers began to notice higher rates of dwelling house burglaries at certain locations and among certain victims. Tracking incident rates over the previous 5 years, police identified six neighborhoods that suffered from consistently high levels of burglary in which money and jewelry appeared to be targeted, and six neighborhoods where more general property was targeted. Three neighborhoods within each group of six were assigned to treatment and three were assigned to control, consistent with a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Police analyzed the environmental structure of these neighborhoods and their consequences based on theories of environmental criminology. They also engaged with community members to gain additional perspective and former offenders to learn about methods used for offending. The police felt that the problem was the result of offenders repeatedly targeting areas that they were familiar with, requiring a change in both the physical structure and the behavior of victims. In response, the project staff distributed crime prevention supplies such as light timers, anti‐climb paint, security lighting, etc. to homes within intervention areas. Police also erected signage advertising neighborhood watch programs and held public meetings discussing the program to engage with residents. The assessment of the project involved comparison of burglary counts for the 5 years preceding the intervention to burglary counts for the year following implementation. Comparison was made between both treatment neighborhoods and their respective comparison areas. The program appeared to be effective based on a greater percentage decline at all target locations relative to comparison locations. It was further suggested that there was no evidence of displacement, though no statistical tests of displacement were presented. The authors also examined the economic feasibility of the program by comparing the cost associated with program implementation, the typical cost of a residential burglary to the victim and the justice system, and the overall reduction in incidents in treatment locations relative to comparison locations. Based on this analysis they conclude that the program was effective in generating cost‐benefit savings that outweighed the price of program implementation.

Elliott ( 2007 ); Reno Police Department ( 2006 )

Reno (NV) police officers assigned to work in the Downtown Tax District noticed abundant crime and disorderly conditions in the area's low‐budget motels. Business owners and tourists began complaining as well, leading to the initiation of a problem‐solving approach addressing motel crime in the area. Officers began conducting surveillance of the most problematic motels, noting any environmental security risks and code violations. The project officers then conducted interviews with suspects, motel residents, motel managers, and school representatives. Through these interviews and analyses of motel registration practices, officers learned that managers were improperly renting rooms and facilitating crime, while the children of tenants were frequently absent from school. Officers determined that changing management, physical security, and partnering with outside agencies to provide resources for families would all be necessary steps. The response phase began with initial warrant sweeps and door‐to‐door contacts at problem motels. Project staff then partnered with local agencies to explain and conduct code enforcement inspections, as well as present CPTED recommendations. Police also worked with motel managers to institute stricter registration procedures and worked with the County Attorney for special prosecution of motel crime offenders. Other response activities included communication with probation and parole to increase supervision of clients in the motel district, working with local community and social services to provide resources for families living in motels (i.e., food and clothing, computer access at local libraries, etc.), and educating motel managers on recognition of criminal behavior. The motels that received the intervention were all located in the Downtown Tax District, known as the Motel Interdiction Team (MIT) zone. Elliott ( 2007 ) assessed the effectiveness of the intervention by using a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Calls for service and crime incident counts were compared before and after the intervention for the 35 motels in the MIT zone and the 30 motels outside the MIT zone (comparison motels). Evaluation of crime data was conducted using a 182‐day preintervention and 181‐day postintervention period, however, incident counts were subsequently dropped from the analysis due to low base rates. Results from the calls for service assessment indicated that there was a decrease in aggregate calls for service in the MIT zone compared with an increase in aggregate calls for service outside the MIT zone, though neither overall change was statistically significant based on t test results. This finding was largely driven by a significant increase in crimes against person calls outside the MIT zone, coupled with a nonsignificant decrease in crimes against person calls inside the MIT zone. Reno Police Department ( 2006 ) also reported that the decrease in calls for service within the MIT zone equated to an estimated savings of 1,750 police officer hours per year.

Gill et al. ( 2018 )

Researchers partnered with police in Seattle, Washington to address street segments of high‐frequency youth‐related crime/disorder. Police data was mapped and incidents involving youth (ages 12–25) as either suspects, arrestees, or victims were examined to identify street‐level concentrations of crime. The hottest street segments were then matched into two pairs based on incident rate and street‐level characteristics. Within the two matched pairs, street segments were randomly assigned to either treatment or control, resulting in the use of a randomized experimental research design. Small groups of officers were assigned to each intervention area to conduct a primarily non‐enforcement based problem‐solving initiative. During the analysis phase, the problem‐solving teams examined official data and CPTED assessments, as well as engaged in conversations with community and business stakeholders. Officers were encouraged to develop responses that focused on strengthening the infrastructure for youth, rather than relying on enforcement. Response activities differed between the two intervention sites, in the Westlake Park area officers partnered with community stakeholders and local government to promote collective efficacy and implement CPTED changes. Officers also worked with social services to provide shelter for a homeless individual that had been the genesis for other issues in the park. In the “Retail Street” intervention area officers initially began with targeted enforcement of adult drug dealers/users. Other responses included rerouted bus lines, improvements to street furniture, and increased cooperation with Metro Transit Police. The assessment was conducted using pre–post incident and calls for service data for both intervention areas, as well as their respective control locations. Poisson regression models indicated that the effectiveness of the program differed by intervention area. In Westlake Park, the intervention was associated with a nonsignificant increase in calls for service relative to the matched control area. Conversely, on Retail Street the intervention was associated with a statistically significant decrease in calls for service relative to the matched control. Findings were similar for crime incidents, with Westlake Park experiencing a respective increase, and Retail Street experiencing a respective decrease, relative to controls (though neither result was statistically significant by conventional standards). Gill et al. ( 2018 ) suggest that the differing effects may be related to the nature of the response activities. In Westlake Park, where enforcement responses were not used, program effects may be delayed, or measurement may not have adequately captured the changing dynamics within the community. On Retail Street, however, enforcement responses may have helped to contribute to short‐term crime reductions that are receptive to measurement.

Groff et al. ( 2015 ); Ratcliffe et al. ( 2015 )

The Philadelphia Policing Tactics experiment compared the effectiveness of POP, foot patrol, and offender‐focused policing in addressing hot spots of violent crime. Hot spots were identified by geocoding and mapping incident data from the prior year. Hot spot boundaries were then determined by District Captains based on their knowledge of the areas, and a total of 81 violent crime hot spots were selected. Police then divided these areas into groups of 27 based on which areas they felt would be best suited for each policing tactic, resulting in 27 potential problem‐solving hot spots. Due to the police department's desire to treat a certain number of total hot spots, randomization was conducted using a 3:1 ratio. Thus, within the group of POP hot spots, 20 locations were randomly assigned to treatment and 7 locations to control (standard policing). Problem‐solving was conducted by POP teams in each treatment location. Analyses and response activities varied by location, but District Captains were required to continually update and submit action plans and progress reports. Some responses included partnership with other agencies and enforcement measures such as focusing on known offenders and foot patrol. Additionally, a number of locations ultimately switched their focus away from violent crime, instead targeting property and drug‐related offenses. This randomized experiment was evaluated through a comparison of all violent crime and violent felony incident counts. Outcomes were measured pre‐ and postintervention for both treatment and control hot spots, as well as across policing tactics. Negative binomial models indicated nonsignificant changes in both violent felonies and all violent crime measures for POP treatment places relative to control. Furthermore, because the effects of the POP intervention were nonsignificant, displacement was not measured for these hot spots. Groff et al. ( 2015 ) suggest that the heterogeneity in targeted problems may have been one of the contributing factors to the lack of measurable violence reduction. Ratcliffe et al. ( 2015 ) also conducted a survey assessment of the intervention's effects on community perceptions. A total of 157 residents from POP locations and 159 residents from control locations were compared during the preintervention period to 162 POP residents and 177 control residents during the postintervention period. Results from OLS regression models indicated that there was no significant effect of the intervention on satisfaction with police or perceptions of violent crime, property crime, physical and social disorder, safety, or procedural justice.

Guseynov ( 2010 )

Guseynov ( 2010 ) evaluated a problem‐solving initiative conducted by the Kansas City Police Department's Comprehensive Strategic Team Accountability Review (CSTAR) unit. The police entered the Columbus Park area intending to target crime and quality of life offenses. They determined that abandoned buildings and public housing projects were major contributors to the crime problem in the area, and that there was a lack of partnership between the police and the community. Police responded by tearing down abandoned buildings that had been linked to drugs and prostitution. They also began prosecuting landlords that were facilitating crime and implementing strict code enforcement in the area. Other response activities included replacing destroyed parking meters to remove excuses for rule breaking, partnering with other law‐enforcement agencies to target drug dealers, removing signs of disorder, and working with community members to increase trust and cooperation with the police. Guseynov evaluated the program using a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Weekly Index I crimes within the three police beats comprising the Columbus Park area (intervention area) were compared with the rest of the Central Patrol division. Comparisons were made for the year before, during, and after the intervention. Results from t test analyses indicated that weekly Index I crimes decreased significantly in both intervention and control areas over the course of the project. Guseynov also constructed an anticipated average for the treatment area based on the reduction rate in the control area. This analysis indicated that the treatment area's average weekly crime count had decreased more than would be anticipated based on the trend in the control group, and that the difference between the observed and anticipated treatment group average was statistically significant.

Hollywood Police Department ( 2015 )

The Hollywood, FL Police Department (2015) began receiving complaints from residents during community meetings over increases in residential burglaries. Analysis of official data confirmed rising residential burglary rates in three particular reporting areas, and at specific times of day. Police first began conducting environmental surveys of residences that had experienced repeated victimization, noting potential CPTED recommendations. They also analyzed the characteristics of arrested offenders and the types of property being stolen in the burglaries, noting trends in both offender profile and targeted property. All units of the department began meeting monthly to discuss the problem‐solving strategies in an iterative fashion. The response phase was initiated with increased patrol presence in the targeted areas during at‐risk times. Police also began making changes to the physical layout of the areas, such as closing or restricting access to alleyways, constructing deterrent signage that advertised the anti‐burglary initiative, implementing a property marking campaign for victims, and conducting security surveys with victims to recommend target hardening/surveillance measures. Other response activities included establishing neighborhood watch programs and partnering with probation to monitor prior offenders. The project assessment was conducted using a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. The three targeted reporting areas were compared with three similarly sized reporting areas that did not receive the intervention. Burglary incident counts were compared before and after the intervention for both groups. This comparison indicated that all target areas experienced decreases in burglary incidents over the course of the intervention, while all comparison locations experienced increases in burglary incidents over the same time span. There was a noted small amount of spatial displacement; however, no statistical tests were provided.

Houston Police Department ( 2012 )

The Antoine corridor in Houston, Texas is described as a one‐mile stretch of road surrounded on both sides by residential apartments. The Houston Police Department ( 2012 ) had been dealing with rising crime and declining occupancy in this area for several years and had made prior attempts at intervention via increased presence and enforcement but had ultimately been unable to address the problem. As the area worsened, local media coverage and citizen complaints mounted, leading the department to pursue a problem‐solving initiative to address the crime problem. Officers believed that the apartment complexes in the area were facilitating issues by renting to criminals. Analysis of crime data confirmed that certain apartment complexes in the area were responsible for disproportionate amounts of crime. Thus, the police began responding to apartment complexes one by one in an iterative SARA loop. Officers first began with strict code enforcement and partnership with other police units to execute warrant sweeps and gather intelligence on gang members. As a result, local government held dangerous building hearings, leading to the demolition of one particularly problematic complex. As police repeated this process targeting new apartment complexes, they were also able to cooperate with several government agencies to pass legislation that allowed greater enforcement and supervision activity over rental properties. Response activities also included engagement with residents and local stakeholders regarding input on area redevelopment, which lead to the construction of new area housing that was CPTED compliant. The intervention was assessed using a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design that compared Part I crime counts for the intervention area and the rest of the city as a whole. Crime counts were provided for several years before and after the intervention. Crime count trends indicated that the intervention area experienced large and sustained decreases in Part I crime and narcotics cases in the postintervention period, declining by as much as 57% from preintervention peaks. The rest of the city also experienced decreases, but of far less magnitude. The authors claim that there was initial evidence of displacement that quickly gave way to diffusion of benefits; however, the report contains no associated statistical test.

Knoxville Police Department ( 2002 )

The Knoxville (TN) Police Department (2002) describes a program designed in response to citizen complaints about repeat offenders. These repeat offenders tended to be parolees or probationers that received limited supervision and services in the community. Working with the Tennessee Board of Probation and Parole, officers reviewed parolee records and citizen complaints, determining that past efforts such as increased patrol (more arrests) and reduced workloads had been largely unsuccessful. They recognized that these offenders re‐entering the community frequently had dysfunctional families and substance abuse and mental health problems. The two agencies created the Knoxville Public Safety Collaborative as a response, combining the resources of the police and probation services and collaborating with 25 human service providers to bring much needed services to parolees. The response involved coordinated and proactive treatment in which the parolee and parole officer developed a release plan, followed by a multi‐division staff meeting to discuss treatment options, and then the parolee supervision by a team including police officers, probation officers, and community service providers. The 265 parolees in the program were compared with a historical comparison group of 261 parolees who would have been eligible for the program. This quasi‐experimental evaluation was completed by the University of Tennessee School of Social Work. Assessment of success rates indicated that 78 (29%) program participants succeeded in not having their parole revoked while only 29 (11%) succeeded in the comparison group. Program participants were also found to be less likely to pick up new charges and receive technical violations.

Kochel and Weisburd ( 2017 ); Kochel and Weisburd ( 2019 ); Kochel et al. ( 2015 )

The St. Louis County Hot Spots in Residential Areas experiment evaluated the differing effects of problem‐solving, directed patrol, and standard policing on hot spots of crime. Part I and Part II crime incident data was mapped to identify both stable and active hot spots. After initial hot spots were identified, precinct commanders evaluated the potential hot spots, and ultimately 71 areas were included in the study. Using a randomized experimental design, hot spots were first separated into four blocks based on shared characteristics, and then randomly assigned from within each block to receive either problem‐solving, directed patrol, or standard policing. Randomization resulted in 20 problem‐solving hot spots and 31 control hot spots. Twenty‐two police officers were assigned to the problem‐solving areas, and problem‐solving officers were also provided a dedicated crime analyst. The analyst would provide incident and calls for service data for officers to examine during problem scanning and analysis processes, and officers were generally found to have focused on problems that accounted for the highest calls for service levels. Problem‐solving efforts were required to involve partnership with an outside stakeholder on at least one problem, and while problems differed by location, responses generally focused on either property crimes, violent crimes, quality of life offenses, or repeat address issues. Analysis data sources often included resident surveys, area observation, CPTED assessments, interviews, and discussions with community members, landlords, businesses, school representatives, etc. Problem responses also varied based on location, but included target hardening education and implementation, nuisance abatement, area cleanup, code enforcement, and communication with other agencies (among others). To evaluate the experiment, Kochel et al. ( 2015 ) used a time‐series analysis to compare trends in weekly calls for service levels before and after implementation of the intervention. ARIMA time‐series models were created for problem‐solving, directed patrol, and control locations. These models indicated that problem‐solving places experienced a statistically significant decline in calls for service per week, while control locations experienced a nonsignificant decline. Kochel et al. also conducted community surveys to assess changes across several community‐level variables. Analysis of survey responses indicated that there were some initial but short‐term decreases in police legitimacy and feelings of safety among residents in problem‐solving areas, but that there were long‐term improvements in willingness to cooperate with police, relative to control locations. Subsequent survey analyses by Kochel & Weisburd ( 2017 ,  2019 ) indicated that residents in problem‐solving areas exhibited delayed but increased willingness to cooperate with police and increased informal social control relative to residents in standard policing communities. However, these residents/areas did not display significant changes in perceptions of procedural justice, police abuse, legitimacy, social cohesion, or collective efficacy.

Lancashire Constabulary ( 2008 )

In 2005 a new neighborhood policing team took over the Farringdon Park area of Preston, England. Official data indicated that Farringdon Park was one of the most deprived neighborhoods in England, and that the area suffered from disproportionately high levels of crime. Concerns over crime and disorder in the area were highlighted by elected representatives, local community, and press reports. Police analyzed official data as well as data from the primary landlord association in the area, determining that criminal damage was becoming a major economic and environmental cost. Surveys were carried out to assess neighborhood residents’ thoughts on crime issues in the area and receptivity to the formation of a community group. The police held meetings with residents and local representatives and carried out environmental surveys with cooperation from service providers and other stakeholders. Project staff also met with the head of criminology at the University of Central Lancashire and reviewed relevant criminological theory. Based on this analysis, the response phase consisted of various enforcement, situational, and social crime prevention tactics. Enforcement measures included warrant execution, high profile arrests, evictions, and hot spot patrol, among others. Situational measures included target hardening of various access points, area cleanup, lighting improvements, CCTV funding, and road redesign. Social measures involved restorative justice, outreach work by youth services, recreational opportunities, and other youth intervention schemes. The project was evaluated with a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Crime incident and calls for service data for the intervention neighborhood were compared with another neighborhood with similar characteristics that did not receive the intervention. Incident and calls for service counts were presented both before and after the intervention. Comparison of raw differences for treatment and control locations indicated that the target area experienced a sizable decrease in crime counts over the course of the intervention, while the comparison area experienced a slight increase. Furthermore, while both areas also experienced a decline in call numbers, the target area's decline was notably larger. The authors assessed displacement/diffusion by examining crime and call rates in an adjoining neighborhood. This analysis suggested no evidence of displacement, but rather evidence of diffusion of benefits. However, no test of statistical significance was included in the report. Monetary savings were also examined through comparison of yearly crime costs. Savings for the postintervention year were reported to be as much as $220,467.

Lancashire Constabulary ( 2012 )

Lancashire police became aware of public concern regarding juvenile crime and delinquency in Preston, England. While the number of first arrests among juveniles was decreasing slightly, police observed a lack of resources for at‐risk youth and were concerned that existing resources would continue to decline due to budget cuts. Thus, police felt that juvenile arrests would begin to increase without proper intervention. In the analysis phase, police examined national trends and economic costs associated with youth offenders. They also analyzed the characteristics of the youth offenders, victims, and locations of offenses within the city, as well as literature on youth interventions. The response drew inspiration from the American “Scared Straight” program, but with a focus on early intervention and education rather than fear. An asset scoring system was created wherein police intervention with youth would trigger the allocation of points for the individual involved. As points accumulated, the youth's record would be reviewed at weekly meetings between police and other project partners. When enough points accumulated, the “Custody Experience” would be initiated. This intervention involved collecting the individual from their home and taking them to the police station. Once there, the youth would be given a scenario of possible arrest and educated about the custody process, consequences, and threats associated with their actions. The intervention was designed to be tailored to the specific circumstances of the individual, and with the intention to provide a positive and preventative experience. The project was assessed using a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Youth reprimand counts for the entire intervention city were compared against two nonintervention cities that were selected based on similar geographic and financial characteristics. Reprimand counts were provided for both pre‐ and postintervention years. Between the year prior to, and the year following, the intervention, reprimand rates in the target city decreased by 33% while reprimand rates in the two comparison cities rose by 11% and 14%, respectively. The authors also suggest that the decrease in reprimands equated to a cost savings of £82,000.

Lexington Division of Police ( 2009 )

In 2006 the former and current Police Chiefs of the Lexington Division of Police commissioned a historical analysis of the department's official crime data. During this review, the department's analysis unit identified seven neighborhoods with disproportionate amounts of reported crime and calls for service. The problems in these neighborhoods were also frequently recognized by local government, residents, and police observations. To address these problematic neighborhoods police began analyzing UCR data, as well as victim and resident surveys to further determine community perceptions and concerns. Police determined that these neighborhoods were primarily multi‐housing units, but that local business operations had been declining, leading to increased vacancy. They further determined that there was a prevailing perception that the majority of offenders resided within the community, rather than outside of it. Additional analysis activities specific to particular neighborhoods included consultation with government agencies, social services, and inspectional services. In the response phase, police conducted meetings with various government and community organizations to discuss strategies and foster communication. Neighborhood response officers began engaging in directed patrols and other proactive techniques focusing on problem offenders. Other response measures included code inspection and enforcement at high crime addresses, construction of deterrent signage, area cleanup, fence, and streetlight repair, and other situational measures. The project assessment was conducted with a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Reported crime counts for the seven intervention neighborhoods were compared with reported crime counts for the rest of the city. Counts were provided for 2 years before and 2 years after the year of project implementation. Trends in the data suggested that reported crime decreased in the seven intervention neighborhoods by an average of 8%, while reported crime in the rest of the city (or neighborhoods not receiving intervention) decreased by only 1%. This finding is also used as evidence that there was no displacement of crime, as the remainder of the city did not experience a subsequent crime increase. However, no statistical test of displacement was included.

London Borough of Enfield ( 2011 )

Enfield's Community Safety Partnership (CSP) noticed an increase in burglary offenses during 2008, after rates had been stable for several years prior. Meetings with residents, community safety surveys, and local media coverage further indicated that burglary was a major concern among the public. The CSP was also concerned with the economic and psychological effects that burglary can create, leading to the implementation of a problem‐solving initiative designed to address it. Project staff mapped and analyzed official data, noticing burglary concentrations in specific hot spots, seasonal burglary trends, and consistencies in the characteristics of the offenses (i.e., method of entry). The partnership similarly assessed the environmental characteristics of the highest frequency areas, noting physical security risks that needed to be addressed. There was also analysis of criminological literature and information on prior burglary offenders in the area, including offenders that had been apprehended and subsequently provided rationale for their offense. Ultimately, the project staff felt that they had an actionable understanding of the environmental characteristics of locations that predisposed them to repeat victimization. Thus, the response primarily involved situational crime prevention measures such as installing window and door locks, controlling access to alleyways, distribution of timer switches, low‐watt bulbs, and shock alarms. Residents were also given the resources to mark property and provided crime prevention literature. Further response activities included publicization of burglary awareness and graffiti and area cleanup. The intervention was assessed using a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Houses that received the intervention were aggregated and compared with all other nonintervention houses in the borough. Burglary incident counts were used to compare the treatment and comparison groups both before and after the intervention. This evaluation indicated that intervention houses experienced 78.7% fewer burglaries in the postintervention period than the preintervention period. Conversely, nonintervention houses only experienced 2.1% fewer burglaries over the same time period. The authors also suggest that, when factoring in the economic and social costs associated with burglary, the intervention was responsible for generating a cost savings of £934,000.

Mazerolle et al. ( 2000 )

Mazerolle et al. ( 2000 ) describe a randomized experiment testing the impact of the Beat Health POP program in Oakland, CA. The Beat Health program was designed to address drugs and disorder at problem addresses/street blocks in the city. Sites were referred to the Beat Health team through hotlines, community meetings, and reviews of calls for service. Half of the sites (50) referred were randomly selected to receive the Beat Health treatment; the other half (50) received normal patrol. The analysis used a blocked design to compare residential and commercial addresses separately. The Beat Health intervention involved a team of one police officer and one police service technician visiting a site to identify and analyze the problem and to make contact with the property owner or place manager to try to address the problems. The police attempted to build a close working relationship with individuals who had a stake in improving the property and tried to provide guidance on crime prevention. The intervention typically involved pressuring third parties (usually the landlord of a problem apartment building or property owner) to make changes to improve property conditions.

The Beat Health team could also use the SMART (Specialized Multi‐Agency Response) Team, made up of city inspectors, to enforce local housing, fire, and safety codes. The team could also instigate legal action against landlords and property owners through civil law. This project used a problem‐oriented approach to third‐party policing: Beat Health teams met with property owners and closely examined problem sites to determine the best course of action to target problems.

Calls for service data were used for the assessment. Comparing call data before the intervention to a 12‐month postintervention period, percentage change and mean assessments indicated that experimental sites exhibited a significant decrease in drug calls, primarily driven by decreases at residential sites. In contrast, control sites experienced a significant increase in drug calls, and this was primarily driven by increases at commercial sites. There were no significant decreases in violent crime, disorder, or property crime calls in either experimental or control groups, though disorder calls in the experimental group did decline significantly at commercial sites relative to residential sites. Mazerolle et al. measured displacement/diffusion by examining drug calls for service within 500‐foot catchment areas around each target address. This analysis revealed statistically significant evidence of spatial displacement into the catchment areas at commercial sites, particularly for the control group. There was also some evidence of diffusion of benefits in the residential catchment areas for the experimental group, however, this did not appear to be statistically significant.

Niagara County Sheriff's Office ( 2011 )

The Niagara County Sheriff was contacted by a New York State Senator's office on behalf of the Newfane Business Association. The association was expressing concerns about economic repercussions associated with increasing crime and disorder along the Main Street corridor in the town of Newfane, New York. The small rural town was suffering from visible drug dealing, vacant buildings, and general area blight. Police began by partnering with community members to increase information sharing. This partnership consisted of five officers and eight civilians representing local school, church, and government organizations. Analysis of official data indeed revealed disproportionate trends in CFS rates, with patterns at specific locations and times. During information sharing meetings, members of the partnership discussed these trends and the characteristics of local suspects and problem addresses. It was further determined that problem areas along Main Street were multi‐unit housing complexes with detached landlords. Based on the information learned, the Sheriff devised a multi‐point action plan that involved intensive police enforcement (zero tolerance methods), curfew enforcement for youth, investigation of suspected drug dealers/users, partnership with landlords to evict drug dealers and clean graffiti, code enforcement, warrant sweeps, and other situational and enforcement based responses. The assessment followed a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design by comparing reported crime counts for Newfane to a similar nearby town that did not receive intervention efforts. Crime counts were compared four weeks prior to, and 4 weeks proceeding, the intervention. Results showed that reported crime decreased by 60% in the intervention town, while reported crime decreased by only 7% in the comparison town.

Nunn et al. ( 2006 )

The Brightwood neighborhood in Indianapolis (IN) had been known to be experiencing problems related to drug trafficking and associated violent crime. In 1995 and 1996 surveys with community residents were conducted. These surveys indicated that, not only was there a concentration of crime in and around the Brightwood area, but that residents overwhelmingly felt that the problem was related to drugs. During problem analysis, officers from the Metro Drug Task Force (MDTF) began observing the Brightwood area, trying to identify drug dealers based information they had received from neighborhood and crime watch associations. Through communication with community members and other district police, MDTF officers were able to compile a list of suspected drug dealers in the area. Officers then began pulling arrest records and criminal histories on these offenders to build intelligence files. The MDTF partnered with the FBI to conduct surveillance, both in‐person and through the use of wiretaps. Once enough evidence had been gathered, MDTF officers began to obtain search, arrest, and seizure warrants. The physical response consisted of a targeted raid and warrant sweep in the Brightwood area, resulting in the arrest of 21 individuals considered to be major drug dealers. To evaluate the interdiction a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design was used. Calls for service data in the Brightwood neighborhood were compared with calls for service data in a nearby neighborhood (Westside). The comparison neighborhood had similar economic characteristics and a similar number of residents between the ages of 14 and 29. Call numbers were measured preintervention and for each of the following 2 years after the intervention. Analysis of t test results indicated that the treatment neighborhood experienced significant decreases in calls for overall serious crime (significant in both postintervention years), burglary (only significant in the second postintervention year), gun crime (both postintervention years), personal violence (both postintervention years), and robbery (both postintervention years). In contrast, the comparison neighborhood only witnessed significant decreases in calls for overall serious crime (limited to first postintervention year) and gun crime (limited to first postintervention year). Results for theft calls were nonsignificant for both groups, while drug calls initially increased in the intervention area and decreased in the control area, though these differences were also nonsignificant (in year two drug calls returned to near preintervention levels in both areas).

San Angelo Police Department ( 2006 )

In 2004, the San Angelo (CA) Police Chief asked officers to determine salient problems facing the community. Through these discussions, an increase in reported forgeries was brought to the Chief's attention. The department believed that the rise in forgeries was likely attributable to repeat offenders with drug addictions, and that, through addressing this problem, other crime and safety issues may also be improved. To analyze the problem, police relied on official data, tracking the increase in forgeries over the prior 2 years. The department initially created a fraud unit that relied on traditional enforcement responses, but this approach proved to be ineffective. Reassessing the issue, the police noted that the primary victims of reported forgeries were businesses rather than individual citizens, and thus switched their focus to proactive intervention with city businesses. The police chief initially held meetings with representatives from the most repeatedly victimized businesses, advising them to implement a customer identification checking procedure. When this approach was met with resistance, it was determined that the more effective response would be to involve the customers. Police developed the “See! It's me!” program designed, primarily, to encourage customers to proactively protect themselves from identity theft. The program consisted of distinct stages: in the public education stage, the threat of identity theft was advertised on television, radio, and billboards. In the retailer training stage, the police provided businesses with program advertisement materials to place in their stores and established a standardized identification checking procedure. In the financial institutions stage, banks and credit unions began advising their customers to limit personal information printed on checks and take other protective measures. Two Walmart stores subsequently embraced the program and were used as the treatment group for impact evaluation. The assessment was quasi‐experimental in nature and compared the two Walmart stores that adopted the program to two other businesses that did not implement the program properly or at all. Reported forgery counts were compared between the two groups for several months before and after program implementation. Both intervention Walmart stores experienced a notable decrease in average reported forgeries per month. In contrast, one of the two comparison businesses reported an increase in average forgeries, and the other remained nearly stable over the same 12‐month time period. Additionally, citywide average monthly forgeries were reported to have decreased by 34% in the 5 months following the evaluation period compared with the previous year average. The authors suggested that offenders were displaced to other businesses as a result of the intervention at Walmart stores; however, no statistical test was discussed or presented.

Sherman et al. ( 1989 )

Sherman and associates (1989) describe the Minneapolis, MN Repeat Call Policing (RECAP) program designed to respond to commercial and residential addresses with a high number of calls for service. Using calls for service data, the top 500 addresses with the most calls were examined. Schools, city hall, hospitals, police stations, parks, check‐cashing locations, and intersections were all removed because police felt these locations were inappropriate for the intervention. The remaining sites were blocked into half commercial (250) and half residential sites (250). These sites were then randomized in rank‐ordered pairs with half of the sites assigned to receive a POP treatment and half to receive standard patrol. After some data cleaning issues, a total of 119 residential sites and 107 commercial sites received the treatment. The treatment team was four officers and a sergeant who were assigned to visit each site and use as many sources as possible to diagnose the problem. These sources included analysis of call data and incident reports, on‐site interviews of residents, and interviews of place managers. Officers were then supposed to design and implement an intervention plan that needed to be approved by the sergeant. The actual treatment varied greatly across addresses. Officers spent a lot of time helping landlords with problem tenants and providing letters to repeat domestic violence victims informing them of their rights and available services. Commercial responses were even more heterogeneous than residential responses. The time spent at each site also varied considerably with officers visiting some addresses only once and others weekly throughout the yearlong intervention period. The program was assessed using a comparison of calls for service data. Pre–post call trends indicated that, in the first 6 months of the program, target residential addresses experienced a statistically significant decrease in calls, however during the second 6 months of the program no call reduction was observed. Additionally, during this second 6‐month period a significant reduction in calls at commercial addresses favored the control group. After the full experimental year, target residential addresses displayed a nonsignificant 6% reduction in calls compared with a 0.1% increase at control addresses, while there were no notable overall differences between treatment and control at commercial addresses.

Stokes et al. ( 1996 )

Stokes and associates (1996) document a POP project designed to reduce student victimization on the way to and from middle school in Philadelphia, PA. Officers recognized that school violence was an issue, and they worked to understand the underlying problems. Using focus groups, victimization surveys, and analysis of police and school data, the police, along with representatives from the Center for Public Policy at Temple University and vice‐principals from Philadelphia middle schools all came to better understand the dynamics of students being attacked on their way to or from school. They used crime mapping to visually display unsafe locations identified by students and the student victimization survey provided data on the level of victimization, how often this victimization was reported, and how dangerous students perceived their trip to and from school to be. Using this data, the Philadelphia Police Department decided to create a police‐secured safe corridor for students to travel on foot safely to one middle school. Using officers from the Philadelphia Police Department, the Temple University Police Department, and the Philadelphia Housing Authority, the police used crime maps to create a corridor 10 blocks long and three blocks wide where police patrols were increased from 8 to 9 am and 2:30 to 4 pm.

During these time periods, two foot patrol officers, a patrol car, and a bike patrolled the corridor.

A pre‐ and poststudent victimization survey was used for the assessment. Student responses in the target middle school were compared with responses from students in three similar middle schools after the 6‐week intervention period. Simple pre–post comparison of survey results revealed that the percentage of students from the test school who reported being attacked increased from 19.4% to 20.2%, while the same measure for students from control schools decreased from 21.2% to 15.2%. Students from test schools also indicated a slight increase in feelings that they would be picked on or bothered (from 32.4% to 33.4%) while control students indicated a slight decrease (from 30.4% to 28.4%). Analysis of variance tests indicated that the increase in reports of being attacked or bothered was nonsignificant in the test school, however, the decrease on the same measure in the control schools was statistically significant. Further, there were no significant differences for either group in fear of being bothered or attacked between pre‐ and postintervention surveys. Results also indicated that there appeared to be little knowledge of the intervention, as only 27.4% of students at the test school reported being aware of the corridor.

Stone ( 1993 )

Stone ( 1993 ) describes a POP project in Atlanta, GA designed to address drug selling and use in public housing projects. Two housing projects were chosen as intervention sites and two were used as comparisons in this quasi‐experiment. To analyze the drug problems, a management team was created with representatives from the Atlanta Police Department and the housing authority. The management team conducted resident victimization surveys to determine the extent of problems and understand resident perceptions of crime problems. The research team, along with the police, conducted extensive research to document the drug problem in the area by examining data from the police, drug treatment facilities, schools, courts, social service agencies, and corrections agencies. The management team focused on five problem areas in the response: poor lighting, abandoned cars, abundant litter, poor playgrounds, and improperly strung clotheslines. These five problems were identified by residents, officers, and supervisors, and the management team thought focusing on these problems would help address some of the underlying issues leading to drug problems. There was also an effort to get uniformed officers to work more closely with undercover narcotics detectives and to have all officers work more cooperatively with the Atlanta Housing Authority. The team did successfully work with Georgia Power to implement weekly lighting checks, abandoned cars were quickly removed, resident cleanup days reduced the litter problem, and dangerously strung clotheslines that could get in the way of officers were quickly repaired. The program was assessed using pre and post victimization data on whether residents in the target and comparison housing projects had been asked to buy or sell drugs. Analysis of variance tests for the year before and after program implementation indicated that residents’ in both treatment and comparison areas reported significant increases in victimization from violent crime and being asked to buy/sell drugs; however, there was a significantly higher increase in treatment sites relative to controls. Conversely, based on difference of proportions tests using official data, there were no significant differences in overall crime and property crime between the target and comparison areas, and the comparison areas experienced an overall significant increase in violent crime and drug arrests relative to the target areas.

Taylor et al. ( 2011 )

Taylor et al. ( 2011 ) conducted a randomized controlled trial of hot spot policing strategies in partnership with the Jacksonville (FL) Sheriff's Office (JSO). With a noted increase in violent crime in recent years, the team mapped official crime data to identify hot spots of street violence (nondomestic violence). This process led to the identification and selection of 83 hot spots with varying land use characteristics. Using block randomization, the hot spots were separated into four groups based on violent incident levels, and randomly assigned to receive either standard policing (control), directed patrol, or POP. In total, 40 areas were assigned to control and 22 areas were assigned to receive the 90‐day POP intervention. A 100‐foot buffer zone was also constructed around each hot spot to measure displacement. The POP areas were provided a total of 60 assigned police officers and four dedicated crime analysts who were to be engaged in problem‐solving activities full‐time. Officers tackled problems in small teams and were advised to determine the root cause of violence in their area. POP teams often worked closely with community members and focused on an array of issues such as specific offenders, the community, and environmental factors. Specific responses varied by area, but common measures included situational strategies (repairing fences and lighting, constructing road barriers) and collaboration with businesses/property managers to address security issues. Other responses involved community surveys and outreach, providing youth with recreational opportunities, area cleanup, nuisance abatement, and code enforcement. Additionally, while some enforcement measures were used, officers were encouraged to focus on preventative responses. To evaluate the experiment, both violent and property crime incident data and calls for service were used. These measures were compared across POP, directed patrol, and control groups both during and after the intervention period. Poisson and negative binomial regression models indicated that nondomestic violence decreased significantly in POP hot spots relative to controls, but only during the postintervention period, and only as measured by incident data. No significant differences were found in either incident counts or calls for service during the intervention period. Additionally, no significant differences were found for property crime, or violent crime as measured by call data, in the postintervention period. Taylor et al. also found statistically significant evidence of displacement of violent calls for service, but no significant evidence of incident displacement. The authors suggest that residents in the buffer areas may have become aware of the POP intervention, resulting in an increased willingness to call the police.

Thomas ( 1998 )

Thomas ( 1998 ) describes the Coordinated Agency Network (C.A.N.) designed to reduce juvenile probationer recidivism in San Diego. The San Diego Police and the San Diego County Probation Department Juvenile Division both recognized that juveniles were frequently being rearrested after release on probation. In San Diego, low‐risk juvenile offenders were typically “banked,” meaning they only had to contact their probation officer by mail. They were largely unsupervised and frequently failed to abide by the conditions of their probation. An analysis of the area revealed that many of these juveniles needed greater supervision because of unstable family lives, and because of their close geographic proximity to major drug ports, gang activity, and a large prison. The police and probation division formed C.A.N. to increase supervision and monitoring of juvenile probationers. Fifteen officers volunteered to help monitor the juveniles and to refer them and their families to community‐based support programs. After an initial assessment by a senior probation officer, police officers assigned to each juvenile would make biweekly visits to be supervisors and mentors. The program included a graduated model of sanctions and rewards based on the juvenile's compliance with probation along with their performance at school. For the assessment, recidivism rates for a group of 80 C.A.N. participants were compared with a group of 80 similar “banked” juveniles who did not participate. Basic comparison of frequencies indicated that individuals in the treatment groups experienced a much lower rate of recidivism (6%) relative to the comparison group (22%). Additionally, 27% of program participants successfully completed probation, compared with 20% in the comparison group.

Tuffin et al. ( 2006 )

Tuffin et al. ( 2006 ) report on the National Reassurance Policing Programme implemented in six wards (neighborhoods) in the United Kingdom. The program was designed to address the “reassurance gap,” the idea that residents are fearful of increasing crime rates even when crime is actually decreasing. This gap has been explained in part by the signal crimes perspective, which argues that certain crimes, particularly certain types of disorder, signal to the community that crime is out of control. Thus, the rates of these signal crimes are more important in generating resident perceptions than actual overall crime rates. The program had three main focuses: having accessible and visible police officers, community involvement in identifying priorities for police, and using targeted police activity and problem solving. A seven‐stage model was used to implement the program: Research ‐ officers had to find out about the neighborhood and how to engage residents; Engage ‐ police needed to create conditions for dialogue; Public preferences‐ officers used surveys, questionnaires, neighborhood meetings, and visual audits to better understand problems facing the community; Investigation and analysis ‐police used meetings and focus groups to give a deeper analysis to identified problems; Public choices ‐ the police presented the findings of their analysis to residents, so the community could choose priorities; Plan and action ‐officers developed and implemented a plan with local partners; Review ‐ police completed an assessment of the problem. The specific problems targeted varied by ward, but all included some type of antisocial behavior, and typically involved drug problems. The researchers used total recorded crime as a method of assessment, comparing each target site to a similar comparison ward before and after the implementation of the program. Evaluation of the 12‐month preintervention and intervention periods indicated that two of six treatment sites experienced significant decreases in reported crime relative to their matched comparison sites. One matched pair demonstrated a significant reduction in favor of the comparison site, and the other three pairs demonstrated nonsignificant changes.

Vancouver Police Department ( 2009 )

The Granville Entertainment District (GED) in Vancouver, Canada is a nightlife area consisting of both businesses and residential complexes. In 2002 a liquor policy was passed that increased the hours of service within the district. Shortly after, the Vancouver Police Department began to observe increased street‐related crime and disorder in the area. The department initially responded by increasing law‐enforcement presence but were unable to stabilize the problem. Negative media portrayal, public complaints, and officer observations all suggested that an alternative solution was needed. Police began by analyzing official crime data, noticing a significant increase in street crime/disorder‐related calls for service in the GED. Official data also indicated that the primary offenders and victims of these incidents were both intoxicated young adults. Officers also conducted environmental observations of the area, noting a lack of transportation options, overcrowded walkways, gang presence, and problem establishments. The department additionally sought input from the Bar Association, a local bar industry group, during the analysis phase. To respond to the problem, police decided to move away from the traditional enforcement measures that had been unproductive in the past. Importance was placed on increasing communication with the local Bar Association, leading to a line restriction at bars after 2:00 am. Proactive police patrol was increased in the area, encouraging officers to be more mobile and interact with patrons. Police also partnered with local radio to advertise safe practices in the GED and partnered with taxi services to increase transportation options. The response redeveloped overcrowded sidewalks by closing off main through streets on weekend nights, and constructed deterrent signage reminding patrons of police presence in the area. While the majority of response activities were proactive, the police did partner with the Firearm Interdiction Team to target gang members in the GED and conducted an undercover operation to monitor liquor law compliance at local bars. The assessment followed a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Calls for service numbers for the GED were compared with two other popular nearby entertainment districts. Comparisons were provided both pre‐ and postintervention. Simple analysis of percentages indicated that the intervention area witnessed a 20% decrease in calls for service after the intervention, relative to before. In contrast, one of the comparison districts experienced a decrease of only 4%, while the other comparison district experienced a 46% increase. Increases in the call rates of nearby areas led the authors to suggest that there was minimal displacement, however no statistical test of displacement was presented.

Weisburd and Green ( 1995 )

Weisburd and Green ( 1995 ) evaluated the Jersey City, NJ Drug Market Analysis Program. The program identified 56 hot spots of high‐activity drug dealing. These hot spots were identified using narcotics sales arrests, drug‐related calls for service, narcotics tip‐line information, and the assessments of narcotics detectives. Half of these hot spots were randomly assigned to a POP treatment and half received routine enforcement that relied primarily on arrest. The cases were randomized in four statistical blocks, based on volume of drug activity. Two‐block catchment areas were constructed around each hot spot for measurement of displacement. The program recognized from the outset the need to assign specific officers to specific hot spots to increase accountability, and the need to allow for a diversity of responses to address the problems at a specific hot spot. The program included a step‐wise process similar to the SARA model. In the planning stage, officers collected data on the physical, social, and criminal characteristics of each area. In the implementation stage, officers coordinated efforts to conduct a crackdown at the hot spot and use other relevant responses to address underlying problems at the hot spot. Finally, in the maintenance stage, officers attempted to maintain the positive impact of the crackdown. To implement the experiment, squads of narcotics officers were randomly assigned to the treatment or control hot spots. The assessment used calls for service data for the 7‐month pre‐ and postintervention periods. An analysis of variance model indicated that a significant difference in overall disorder‐related calls favored the treatment group. While both treatment and control areas experienced increases, the control areas’ increase was significantly greater. The model showed no significant differences between the two groups on violent or property calls, and the measurement of narcotics calls was deemed to be unreliable and omitted from the results. Displacement analysis results suggested a diffusion of benefits into experimental catchment areas for both public morals and narcotics calls. The emergence of new hot spots was also determined to be nearly two times more likely to occur in control catchment areas than experimental catchment areas.

Zidar et al. ( 2017 )

During an interview for a crime analyst position with the Paducah (KY) Police Department (PPD), Zidar was asked to analyze local crime data. During the process, Zidar noticed that incidents at local Walmart stores were expending a disproportionate amount of officer time and resources. After being hired, the PPD asked Zidar to pursue this issue and he began riding with patrol officers, talking with community members, and examining official crime data. He determined that Walmart stores were accounting for a disproportionate number of shoplifting incidents and that there was a sense within the department that little could be done. During analysis, the characteristics of shoplifting offenses were examined, and the physical layout of the stores was assessed. The analysis suggested there were too few elements of guardianship or natural surveillance in the stores, and that the result was an environment conducive to theft. Police representatives met with Walmart management and provided recommendations for redesign of the physical environment and the management practices. Based on these meetings, it was determined that Walmart management felt little responsibility for the prevention of theft, and that other response options would need to be pursued. In response, the PPD altered the reporting practices for shoplifting incidents at the two local Walmart locations. They created an online reporting system that was to be used by Walmart loss prevention for any shoplifting incidents in which the reported value stolen was less than $500. This system forced the store to bypass the PPD and take these cases directly to the County Attorney, rather than receive police response (unless violence was used, or loss‐prevention could not determine the identity of the offender). This response was an effort to force Walmart management to take responsibility, and simultaneously free up police resources. Walmart subsequently coordinated with a third‐party vendor to implement a restorative justice program giving offenders the option of bypassing formal case‐processing. The evaluation of the program followed a nonequivalent control group quasi‐experimental design. Reported larcenies under $500 were measured for the two Walmart stores in the intervention area and compared with four nearby Walmart stores, and a nearby mall, that did not receive the online reporting system. Independent t tests indicated that reported larcenies decreased significantly at the intervention Walmart stores between pre‐ and postintervention periods. One comparison location also witnessed a significant decrease, while another witnessed a significant increase, in reported larcenies. The other three stores experienced nonsignificant changes. Zidar et al. ( 2017 ) also conducted a cost/benefit analysis, comparing average monthly time and monthly cost associated with theft‐related calls at local Walmart stores. Results indicated a statistically significant decrease in mean monthly cost and time related to these calls during the postintervention period relative to the preintervention period .

APPENDIX B. GPD SYSTEMATIC SEARCH STRATEGY

Search terms.

To ensure optimum sensitivity and specificity, the GPD search strategy utilizes a combination of free‐text and controlled vocabulary search terms. Because controlled vocabularies and search capabilities vary across databases, the exact combination of search terms and field codes are adapted to each database.

The free‐text search terms for the GPD are provided in Table  B1 and are grouped by substantive (i.e., some form of policing) and evaluation terminology. Although the search strategy may vary slightly across search locations, it follows a number of general rules:

  • Search terms are combined into search strings using Boolean operators “AND” and “OR.” Specifically, terms within each category are combined with “OR,” and categories will be combined with “AND.” For example: (police OR policing OR “law#enforcement”) AND (analy* OR ANCOVA OR ANOVA OR …).
  • Compound terms (e.g., law enforcement) are considered single terms in search strings by using quotation marks (i.e., “law*enforcement”) to ensure that the database searches for the entire term rather than separate words.
  • Wild cards and truncation codes are used for search terms with multiple iterations from a stem word (e.g., evaluation, evaluate) or spelling variations (e.g., evaluat* or randomi#e).
  • If a database has a controlled vocabulary term that is equivalent to “POLICE,” the term is combined in a search string that includes both the policing and evaluation free‐text search terms. This approach ensures that the search retrieves documents that do not use policing terms in the title/abstract but have been indexed as being related to policing in the database. An example of this approach is the following search string: (((SU: “POLICE”) OR (TI,AB,KW: police OR policing OR “law*enforcement”)) AND (TI,AB,KW: intervention* OR evaluat* OR compar* OR …)).
  • For search locations with limited search functionality, a broad search that uses only the policing free‐text terms is implemented.
  • Multidisciplinary database searches are limited to relevant disciplines (e.g., include social sciences but exclude physical sciences).
  • Search results are refined to exclude specific types of documents that are not suitable for systematic reviews (e.g., newspapers, front/back matter, book reviews).

Free‐text search terms for the GPD systematic search

Search Locations

To reduce publication and discipline bias, the GPD search strategy adopts an international scope and involves searching for literature across a number of disciplines (e.g., criminology, law, political science, public health, sociology, social science and social work). The search captures a comprehensive range of published (i.e., journal articles, book chapters, books) and unpublished literature (e.g., working papers, governmental reports, technical reports, conference proceedings, dissertations) by implementing a search strategy across bibliographic/academic, gray literature, and dissertation databases or repositories.

It is noted that there is substantial overlap of the content coverage between many of the databases. Therefore, the Optimal Searching of Indexing Databases (OSID) computer program (Neville & Higginson, 2014 ) has been used to analyze the content cross‐over for all databases that have accessible content coverage lists. OSID analyses the content coverage and creates a search location solution that provides the most comprehensive coverage via the least number of databases. Another advantage of using OSID when designing a search strategy is the reduction in the number of duplicates that would need to be removed prior to the screening phase. Databases with >10 unique titles are searched in full, whereas databases with ≤10 unique titles were searched only the unique titles and any non‐serial content (e.g., reports, conference proceedings). Where a modified search of a database would be more labor‐intensive than a full search and export results, a full search of the database is conducted. The final search locations and solutions are reported in Table  B2 .

GPD search locations and protocol (January 1, 1950–December 2018)

APPENDIX C. GPD SYSTEMATIC COMPILATION STRATEGY

Inclusion criteria.

Each record captured by the GPD systematic search must satisfy all inclusion criteria to be included in the GPD: timeframe, intervention and research design. There are no restrictions applied to the types of outcomes, participants, settings or languages considered eligible for inclusion in the GPD.

Types of interventions

Each document must contain an impact evaluation of a policing intervention. Policing interventions are defined as some kind of a strategy, program, technique, approach, activity, campaign, training, directive, or funding/organizational change that involves police in some way (other agencies or organizations can be involved). Police involvement is broadly defined as:

  • Police initiation, development or leadership
  • Police are recipients of the intervention or the intervention is related, focused or targeted to police practices
  • Delivery or implementation of the intervention by police

Types of study designs

The GPD includes quantitative impact evaluations of policing interventions that utilize randomized experimental (e.g., RCTs) or quasi‐experimental evaluation designs with a valid comparison group that does not receive the intervention. The GPD includes designs where the comparison group receives “business‐as‐usual” policing, no intervention or an alternative intervention (treatment‐treatment designs).

The specific list of research designs included in the GPD are as follows:

  • Systematic reviews with or without meta‐analyses
  • Cross‐over designs
  • Cost‐benefit analyses
  • Regression discontinuity designs
  • Designs using multivariate controls (e.g., multiple regression)
  • Matched control group designs with or without preintervention baseline measures (propensity or statistically matched)
  • Unmatched control group designs with pre–post intervention measures which allow for difference‐in‐difference analysis
  • Unmatched control group designs without preintervention measures where the control group has face validity
  • Short interrupted time‐series designs with control group (less than 25 preintervention and 25 postintervention observations)
  • Long interrupted time‐series designs with or without a control group (≥25 pre‐ and postintervention observations)
  • Raw unadjusted correlational designs where the variation in the level of the intervention is compared with the variation in the level of the outcome

The GPD excludes single group designs with pre‐ and postintervention measures as these designs are highly subject to bias and threats to internal validity.

Systematic Screening

To establish eligibility, records captured by the GPD search progress through a series of systematic stages which are summarized in Figure  C1 , with additional detail provided in the following subsections.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-16-e1089-g015.jpg

GPD systematic compilation process

All research staff working on the GPD undergo standardized training before beginning work within any of the stages detailed below. Staff then complete short training simulations to enable an assessment of their understanding of the GPD protocols and highlight any areas for additional training. In addition, random samples of each staff's work are regularly cross‐checked to ensure adherence to protocols. Disagreements about screening decisions between staff are mediated by either the project manager or GPD chief investigators.

Title and abstract screening

After removing duplicates, the title and abstract of records captured by the GPD systematic search is screened by trained research staff to identify potentially eligible research that satisfies the following criteria:

  • Document is dated between 1950 and present
  • Document is unique (i.e., not a duplicate)
  • Document is about police or policing
  • Document is an eligible document type (e.g., not a book review)

Records are excluded if the answer to any one of the criteria is unambiguously “No,” and will be classified as potentially eligible otherwise. Records classified as eligible at the title and abstract screening stage progress to full‐text document retrieval and screening stages.

Full‐text eligibility screening

Wherever possible, a full‐text electronic version of an eligible record is imported into SysReview (review management software; Higginson & Neville, 2014 ). For records without an electronic version, a hardcopy of the record is located to enable full‐text eligibility screening. The full‐text of each document is screened to identify studies that satisfy the following criteria:

  • Document is unique
  • Document reports a quantitative statistical comparison
  • Document reports on policing evaluation
  • Document reports in a quantitative impact evaluation of a policing intervention
  • Evaluation uses an eligible research design

APPENDIX D. LIST OF POP EXPERTS CONTACTED

List of policing scholars and practitioners contacted to identify any studies we missed (Note: Job titles reflect employer as of January 2020)

APPENDIX E. CODING SHEET

POP META ANALYSIS CODING SHEET

Reference Information

  • 1. Document ID: __ __ __ __
  • 2. Study author(s): ____________________
  • 3b. If yes, list secondary/additional study author(s):___________
  • 3c. Study title(s) associated with intervention: _______________________
  • 2. Book chapter
  • 3. Journal article (peer reviewed)
  • 4. Thesis or doctoral dissertation
  • 5. Government report (state/local)
  • 6. Government report (federal)
  • 7. Police department report
  • 8. Technical report
  • 9. Conference paper
  • 10. Award submission
  • 11. Other (specify)
  • 4b. Specify (Other)_____________________
  • 5. Publication date (year): ______________
  • 6a. Journal Name: ____________________
  • 6b. Journal Volume: _______________
  • 6c. Journal Issue: ____________
  • Start: ____________
  • Finish: ____________
  • 8. Source of funding for study: ___________________
  • 9. Country of publication: ___________________
  • 10. Date coded: ___________
  • 11. Coder's Initials: __ __ __

Describing the Problem(s)

  • 1. Crime analysis unit
  • 2. Citizen meeting/organization
  • 3. Officer observation/suggestion
  • 4. Other government agency
  • 5. Funding agency
  • 6. Researcher
  • 7. Other (specify)
  • 12b. Specify (Other) _____________
  • 1. Residential
  • 2. Recreational (bars, restaurants, parks)
  • 3. Hotels/Motels
  • 6. Industrial
  • 7. Agricultural
  • 8. Education
  • 9. Human service (jails, courts, hospitals)
  • 10. Public ways
  • 11. Transport (buses, airports)
  • 12. Open/transitional (construction sites, abandoned buildings)
  • 13. Citywide/no particular environment specified
  • 1. Predatory crimes against persons (sexual assault, robbery, homicide)
  • 2. Predatory crimes against property (vandalism, auto theft)
  • 3. Illegal service crimes (prostitution, selling drugs)
  • 4. Public disorder crimes (disorderly conduct, drunkenness)
  • 5. Vehicular/traffic offenses
  • 6. Status crimes
  • 7. Hard drug use
  • 8. White collar crime (forgery, embezzlement etc.)
  • 9. Overall crime/disorder
  • 10. Other (specify)
  • 14b. Specify (Other) ___________
  • 15. Specifically, what event(s) makes up the problem(s)? ______________________________________________________________________________
  • 1. Offenders
  • 2. Victims/targets
  • 3. Guardians or managers
  • 4. Places/geographic areas
  • 1. Official crime data
  • 2. Arrest information
  • 3. Surveys of people (non‐offenders)
  • 4. Surveys of places or environments
  • 5. Interviews and discussions with people (non‐offenders)
  • 6. Interviews of offenders
  • 7. Literature examination
  • 8. Consultation with government agencies
  • 9. Consultations with businesses
  • 10. Consultations with community organizations
  • 11. Consultations with community members
  • 12. Other (specify)
  • 17b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. No analysis
  • 2. Shallow or cursory analysis (looked at official data)
  • 3. Moderate analysis (looked at official data with analysis by time of day, day of week etc.)
  • 4. In‐depth analysis (3 above, as well as other problem analysis with other data)
  • 5. Authors do not provide sufficient detail to make an assessment

Describing the Response

  • 1. Micro place (e.g., hot spot)
  • 2. Meso area (e.g., neighborhoods)
  • 3. Large area (e.g., entire city)
  • 4. Individual offender
  • 5. Individual victim
  • 6. Group of offenders (e.g., gang)
  • 7. Group of victims
  • 8. Individual guardian or manager
  • 9. Group of guardians or managers
  • 10. Entire population (no types of individuals or groups specified)
  • 8. Individual guardian
  • 9. Group of guardians
  • 21. Briefly describe the response(s) implemented

____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________

  • 1. Increasing the effort of crime (target hardening)
  • 2. Increasing the risks of crime
  • 3. Reducing the rewards of crime
  • 4. Reducing provocations
  • 5. Removing excuses for crime
  • 6. Situational crime prevention used, but specific techniques not specified
  • 7. Unclear/Not mentioned (cannot be sure if SCP was used or not)
  • 8. N/A‐ Situational crime prevention not used
  • 22b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. Neighborhood associations/organizations
  • 2. Government organizations/agencies
  • 3. Social service agencies
  • 4. Commercial establishments/businesses
  • 5. National organizations with an interest in the problem (e.g. MADD)
  • 6. Schools/Academic organizations
  • 7. Individual residents
  • 8. Other police agencies
  • 9. Other criminal justice agencies
  • 23b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. Department wide
  • 2. Multiple precincts or sectors
  • 3. One precinct, sector, or district involved
  • 4. Special units (i.e. community policing unit) involved
  • 5. Select few officers in specific area involved
  • 6. Other (specify)
  • 7. N/A (not mentioned)
  • 24b. Specify (Other)___________________

Implementation of Response

  • 1. There were no reported implementation issues
  • 2. There were minor implementation issues
  • 3. There were more substantial implementation issues
  • 4. There were major implementation issues/the project was not implemented as planned
  • 5. Unclear/no process evaluation included
  • 26. If the process evaluation indicated there were problems with implementation of the response, describe these problems:

__________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________

Location of the intervention

  • 27. Country where study was conducted: __________________
  • 28. City (and state/province, if applicable) where study was conducted: _________________

The following questions refer to the area receiving treatment :

  • 1. Micro place (street segments/blocks)
  • 2. Neighborhood/police beat
  • 3. Police district/precinct
  • 4. Entire city
  • 5. Other (specify)
  • 29b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 30. What is the exact geographic area receiving treatment? _____________________________

The following refer to the area not receiving treatment

  • 31b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 32. What is the exact geographic area not receiving treatment? ___________________________

Methodology/Research design:

  • 33a. Length of pre‐intervention study period______
  • 33b. Length of intervention study period_________
  • 33c. Length of post‐intervention study period______
  • 1. Randomized experiment
  • 2. Nonequivalent control group (quasi‐experimental)
  • 3. Multiple time series (quasi‐experimental)
  • 4. Interrupted time series
  • 35b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. Propensity score matching
  • 2. Identification of matching areas or persons through regression analyses
  • 3. Statistical tests of mean differences among demographic and other relevant variables
  • 4. Comparison of descriptive statistics with no statistical test of differences across groups
  • 5. Comparison to the rest of a jurisdiction or population that did not receive the treatment
  • 35d. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. Extraneous events or factors occurring during the intervention period; historical artifacts
  • 2. Selection of treatment area based on high baseline crime rate
  • 3. Measurement confounds (measure changes over time)
  • 4. Differential attrition, breakdown of randomization, or contamination of control group
  • 5. Pre‐test analyses indicated nonequivalence between treatment and control groups
  • 6. Statistical analyses failed to adjust for nonequivalence at baseline
  • 7. Inappropriate statistical analysis for design
  • 8. Any outcomes measured by reporters that did not have corresponding outcome measures in the results
  • 9. Other threats to internal validity (specify)
  • 36c. Explain any yes responses checked in 34b.

______________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________

  • 38b. If yes, explain ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________
  • 3. Mixed (i.e. varies across sites or analyses)
  • 39b. If no, explain the discrepancy: ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________

Outcomes reported (Note that for each outcome, a separate coding sheet is required)

  • 40. How many crime/disorder outcomes are reported in the study? ____
  • 41. What is the specific outcome recorded on this coding sheet?

_______________________________________________________________

  • 3. Can't tell/researcher did not prioritize outcomes

Dependent Variable

  • 1. Official data (from the police)
  • 2. Researcher observations
  • 3. Self‐report surveys
  • 4. Other (specify)
  • 43b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. Calls for service (911 calls)
  • 3. Incident reports
  • 4. Level of citizen complaints
  • 6. N/A (official data not used)
  • 44b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. Physical observations (e.g. observed urban blight, such as trash, graffiti)
  • 2. Social observations (e.g. observed disorder, such as loitering, public drinking)
  • 3. Other observations (specify)
  • 4. N/A (researcher observations not used)
  • 45b. Specify (Other)___________________
  • 1. Residents/community members
  • 2. Business owners
  • 3. Elected officials
  • 4. Government/social service agencies
  • 6. N/A (self‐report surveys not used)
  • 46b. Specify (Other)___________________

Effect size/Reports of statistical significance

Sample size

  • 47. Based on the unit of analysis for this outcome, what is the total sample size in the analysis? ________
  • 48. What is the total sample size of the treatment group (group that receives the response)? _______
  • 49. What is the total sample size of the control group (if applicable)? _____
  • 50b. If attrition was a problem, provide details (e. g. how many cases lost and why they were lost).
  • 3. Geographic areas
  • 51b. Specify (other) ________________

Effect Size Data

  • 1. Treatment group
  • 2. Control group
  • 3. Neither (exactly equal)
  • 9. Cannot tell (or statistically insignificant report only)
  • 3. N/A (no testing completed/reported)
  • 4. N/A (no p‐value reported)
  • 1. One‐tailed
  • 2. Two‐tailed
  • 9. Cannot tell (unclear from text or not reported)
  • 54b. What type of effect size was reported? _______________
  • 55. If yes, what was the effect size? ______
  • 56. If yes, page number where effect size data is found ________
  • 1. Means and standard deviations
  • 2. t ‐value or F ‐value
  • 3. Chi‐square (df=1)
  • 4. Frequencies or proportions (dichotomous)
  • 5. Frequencies or proportions (polychotomous)
  • 58b. Specify (other) _________

Pre‐post Study Counts

  • 59a. Pre‐period number of events for current outcome in target area _______
  • 59b. During intervention‐period number of events for current outcome in target area ______
  • 59c. Post‐period number of events for current outcome in target area ______
  • 59d. Pre‐period number of events for current outcome in comparison area _______
  • 59e. During intervention‐period number of events for current outcome in comparison area _____
  • 59f. Post‐period number of events for current outcome in comparison area ______

Means and Standard Deviations

  • 60a. Pre‐period treatment group mean. _____
  • 60b. During intervention‐period treatment group mean_____
  • 60c. Post‐period treatment group mean_____
  • 60d. Pre‐period control group mean. _____
  • 60e. During intervention‐period control group mean_____
  • 60f. Post‐period control group mean_____
  • 61a. Pre‐period treatment group standard deviation. _____
  • 61b. During intervention‐period treatment group standard deviation. ____
  • 61c. Post‐period treatment group standard deviation. _____
  • 61d. Pre‐period control group standard deviation. _____
  • 61e. During intervention‐period control group standard deviation. _____
  • 61f. Post‐period control group standard deviation. ______

Proportions or frequencies

  • 62a. n of treatment group with a successful outcome. _____
  • 62b. n of control group with a successful outcome. _____
  • 63a. Proportion of treatment group with a successful outcome. _____
  • 63b. Proportion of control group with a successful outcome. _____
  • 64. IRR value _______

Significance Tests

  • 65a. t ‐value _____
  • 65b. F ‐value _____
  • 65c. Chi‐square value ( df =1) _____

Calculated Effect Size

  • 66a. Effect size ______
  • 66b. Standard error of effect size _____

Mediation Analysis (Note that for each mediator/outcome, a separate coding sheet is required)

  • 68a. What is the specific mediator recorded on this coding sheet?

________________________________________________________________________

  • 68b. Coefficient from treatment to mediator ______
  • 69a. What is the outcome of the mediational analysis?
  • 69b. Coefficient from mediator to outcome______

Cost‐Benefit Analysis (Note that for each outcome, a separate coding sheet is required)

  • 1. Police personnel hours spent on problem
  • 2. Officer time spent attending CFS/problem incidents
  • 3. Cost associated with CFS/problem incidents
  • 4. Other/city costs (specify)
  • 70c. Specify (other) ______________
  • 71. What is the specific cost‐benefit outcome captured on this coding sheet? ________________________________________________________________________
  • 72. Cost of implementing response. _______
  • 73a. Pre‐intervention number of hours spent on identified problem. _______
  • 73b. Post‐intervention number of hours spent on identified problem. _______
  • 74a. Average pre‐intervention personnel hours spent on identified problem. _______
  • 74b. Average post‐intervention personnel hours spent on identified problem. _______
  • 75a. Average yearly pre‐intervention cost attributed to CFS/problem incidents. ______
  • 75b. Average yearly post‐intervention cost attributed to CFS/problem incidents. ______
  • 75c. Total pre‐intervention cost attributed to CFS/problem incidents. _______
  • 75d. Total post‐intervention cost attributed to CFS/problem incidents. _______
  • 76a. Police cost associated with single problem incident. ______
  • 76b. Pre‐intervention treatment group cost. ______
  • 76c. Pre‐intervention control group cost. ______
  • 76d. Post‐intervention treatment group cost. ______
  • 76e. Post‐intervention control group cost. _______

For the following questions please consider the cost of implementing the response (if applicable)

  • 77a. Treatment group cost savings. _____
  • 77b. Control group cost savings. _____
  • 78. Total departmental cost savings._____

Conclusions made by the author(s)

Note that the following questions refer to conclusions about the effectiveness of the intervention in regards to the current outcome/problem being addressed on this coding sheet .

  • 1. The authors conclude intervention associated with a crime decline
  • 2. The authors conclude intervention not associated with a crime decline
  • 3. Mixed across crime/disorder type
  • 4. Unclear/no conclusion stated by authors
  • 3. No statistical test but authors claim evidence of displacement
  • 4. No statistical test but authors claim no evidence of displacement
  • 5. Mixed across crime/disorder type
  • 6. Not tested
  • 81b. If yes, specify what types of displacement were found
  • 82. Additional notes about conclusions:
  • 83. Additional notes about study:

APPENDIX F. LIST OF EXCLUDED STUDIES

Hinkle JC, Weisburd D, Telep CW, Petersen K. Problem‐oriented policing for reducing crime and disorder: An updated systematic review and meta‐analysis . Campbell Syst Rev . 2020; 16 :e1089. 10.1002/cl2.1089 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Systematic Review

Plain language summary on the Campbell website .

1 We note that SARA is not the only model for carrying out POP. Other models, most being refinements/elaborations to SARA, have been introduced over the years including PROCTOR, The 5Is, the 9‐State Model and ID Partners. Sidebottom and Tilley ( 2011 ) provide a review and study of these approaches and note that SARA remains the dominant model. It is important to note that our review did not require explicit mention of the SARA model and programs using any of the models listed above or other similar approaches to carry out the basic tenets of POP were eligible for inclusion.

2 A series of systematic reviews of “hot spots policing” has been conducted by Anthony Braga and colleagues ( 2001 ,  2007 ,  2014 ,  2019 ). Hot spots policing focuses on small geographic areas and concentrations of crime. Hot spots policing per se does not demand detailed analysis of the problem identified and often relies on a law enforcement response. POP can focus on small geographic areas (hot spots); however, further analysis is undertaken to determine the creation of the hot spot and responses are tailored to the needs of each hot spot. Further, POP also examines non‐geographic concentrations of crime—repeat offenders, repeat victims, hot products, and so forth. In short, while POP at hot spots can be considered a type of POP, many hot spots policing programs do not use the more systematic methods associated with POP.

3 The Tilley Award went on hiatus after 2010 and was recently resumed by the Problem Solving and Demand Reduction Programme hosted by the South Yorkshire Police. The most recent winner was announced in March 2019.

4 Seventy‐three percent of departments serving 250,000–499,999 citizens reported encouraging problem solving among officers (Reaves,  2015 ). The same was true for 71% of departments serving 500,000–999,999 and 57% of those serving a million or more residents.

5 However, we note that many of the studies reviewed in these areas employed relatively weak designs (Clarke,  1997 ; Eck,  2002 ; Weisburd,  1997 ).

6 As with our original review (Weisburd et al.,  2008 ,  2010 ) we opted not to include pulling levers/focused deterrence approaches. The original Boston Gun Project (Braga, Kennedy, Waring, & Piehl,  2001 ), which can be viewed as a POP approach that would meet our SARA‐based intervention criteria, only made comparisons to other cities as it was a citywide program. We deemed this did not meet our methodological criteria. Most other pulling‐lever approaches have largely replicated all or parts of the original model rather than going through the SARA process to identify a specific problem and develop a tailor‐made response. Thus, they do not meet our intervention criteria. Additionally, there is a recently updated Campbell systematic review of these programs, which summarizes their impacts on crime (Braga, Turchan, et al.,  2019 ).

7 The seminal pieces that were searched for are: Braga, Weisburd, et al. ( 1999 ), Eck and Spelman ( 1987 ), Goldstein ( 1979 ,  1990 ), Spelman and Eck ( 1987 ), Weisburd et al. ( 2008 ,  2010 ).

8 These journals included: Cambridge Journal of Evidence‐Based Policing, Criminology, Criminology & Public Policy, Justice Quarterly, Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, Journal of Criminal Justice, Police Quarterly, Policing, Police Practice and Research, British Journal of Criminology, Journal of Quantitative Criminology, Crime & Delinquency, Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, Policing and Society: An International Journal of Research and Policy .

9 The POP Center is included in the GPD's gray literature searches, but given the centrality of the center to the current review topic we opted to double check this source for eligible studies.

10 Note we used a slightly different adjustment for over‐dispersion in our original review (Weisburd et al.,  2008 ,  2010 ). We opted to use the current approach for the update to be consistent with more recent Campbell reviews, including Braga and colleagues ( 2019 ) recently updated review of hot spots policing (which includes a handful of overlapping studies/outcomes in cases of POP approaches evaluated at hot spots). Additionally, David Wilson (personal communication) has recently developed a new approach for a more robust adjustment for over‐dispersion when place‐based studies report crime counts for multiple treatment and control areas (or multiple time periods within each group). Unfortunately, we were unable to take advantage of this with our data as too few studies reported on more than one treatment or control area (or only provided aggregated counts by area). For those that did provide disaggregated counts, it was often only two or three areas per group, which is not a sufficient sample for a reasonable adjustment for over‐dispersion in our view.

11 One study (Groff et al.,  2015 ) provided the log IRR and its standard error. For three studies, the effect sizes are risk ratios rather than RIRRs. Two studies reported on probation/parole success and failures in a treatment and control group (Knoxville Police Department,  2002 ; Thomas,  1998 ) and one reported on students being attacked or not in a treatment and control school (Stokes et al., 1996 ). While these are calculated slightly differently, they are comparable to RIRR and interpreted in the same manner. The risk ratios were calculated as follows. Referring to the RIRR two‐by‐two grid above, here a and b are successes (or not being attacked) in the treatment and control group, respectively. Similarly, c and d are the failures (or being attacked) in the two groups. In the traditional notation for risk ratios (see Wilson, in progress ), the treatment group successes are indicated by T and the control group successes are labeled C , while N T and N C are the sample sizes (successes plus failures) in the treatment and control groups, respectively. RR is then calculated as ( T  ×  N C )/ (C  ×  N T ) and its log is the effect size in our models. The variance of log RR is calculated as ( N T  −  T )/ T  ×  N T ) + ( N C  −  C )/ C  ×  N C ) and its square root is the standard error for the log RR.

12 This 2019 publication is included as the “online first” version came out in 2018 and was included in our search results.

13 Our findings here differ somewhat from those of the National Academy of Sciences Committee on Proactive Policing. Weisburd and Majmundar ( 2018 , p. 209) concluded “studies show consistent small‐to‐moderate, positive impacts of problem‐solving interventions on short‐term community satisfaction with the police.” This difference is likely because of our focus on these outcomes only among our eligible studies, which all had a primary crime‐control emphasis. Thus, we caution that our findings here do not necessarily reflect all problem‐solving projects, and in particular do not include studies that did not meet our other eligibility criteria.

REFERENCES TO INCLUDED STUDIES (INCLUDING SUPPLEMENTAL PUBLICATIONS)

  • Anaheim Police Department . (2009). Anaheim Police Department's GRIP on gangs: Gang reduction and intervention partnership: An early gang prevention problem solving strategy . Anaheim, CA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baker, T. , & Wolfer, L. (2003). The crime triangle: Alcohol, drug use, and vandalism . Police Practice and Research , 4 ( 1 ), 47–61. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bichler, G. , Schmerler, K. , & Enriquez, J. (2013). Curbing nuisance motels: An evaluation of police as place regulators . Policing: An International Journal , 36 ( 2 ), 437–462. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bond, B. J. , & Hajjar, L. M. (2013). Measuring congruence between property crime problems and response strategies: Enhancing the problem‐solving process . Police Quarterly , 16 ( 3 ), 323–338. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boston Police Department . (2008). District D‐14: Breaking and entering solution plan . Boston, MA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boston Police Department . (2012). Safe street teams problem‐oriented policing initiative . Boston, MA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. (1997). Solving violent crime problems: An evaluation of the Jersey City police department's pilot program to control violent places . Rutgers University. (Ph.D. dissertation). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , & Bond, B. J. (2008). Policing crime and disorder hot spots: A randomized controlled trial . Criminology , 46 ( 3 ), 577–607. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , & Bond, B. J. (2009). Community perception of police crime prevention efforts: Using interviews in small areas to evaluate crime reduction strategies. In Knutsson J. & Tilley N. (Eds.), Crime Prevention Studies ( 24 , pp. 87–119). Monsey: Criminal Justice Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , Weisburd, D. L. , Waring, E. J. , Mazerolle Green, L. , Spellman, W. , & Gajewski, F. (1999). Problem‐oriented policing in violent crime places: A randomized controlled experiment . Criminology , 37 ( 3 ), 541–580. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carson, J. V. , & Wellman, A. P. (2018). Problem‐oriented policing in suburban low‐income housing: A quasi‐experiment . Police Quarterly , 21 ( 2 ), 139–170. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charlotte‐Mecklenburg Police Department . (2007). Operation safe storage . Charlotte, NC: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chula Vista Police Department . (2009). Reducing crime and disorder at motels and hotels in Chula Vista, California . Chula Vista, CA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooley, W. , Bemiller, M. , Jefferis, E. , & Penix, R. (2019). Neighborhood by neighborhood: Community policing in a rust belt city . Policing: An International Journal , 42 ( 2 ), 226–239. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dario, L. M. (2016). Crime at convenience stores: Assessing an in‐depth problem‐oriented policing initiative (Ph.D. dissertation). Tempe, AZ: Arizona State University.
  • Dayton Police Department . (2011). The urban high school disorder reduction project: Restoring safe schools and inspiring academic excellence . Dayton, OH: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Durham Constabulary . (2017). Reducing dwelling burglaries in areas which repeatedly suffer high rates in county Durham, UK . County Durham: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elliott, M. (2007). An evaluation of specialized police response teams on motel crime (Master's thesis). Reno, NV: University of Nevada.
  • Gill, C. , Weisburd, D. , Vitter, Z. , Shader, C. G. , Nelson‐Zagar, T. , & Spain, L. (2018). Collaborative problem‐solving at youth crime hot posts: A pilot study . Policing: An International Journal , 41 ( 3 ), 325–338. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Glendale Police Department . (2016). Employing problem‐oriented policing to target convenience store crime: The Glendale, Arizona Smart Policing Initiative . Glendale, AZ: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Groff, E. R. , Ratcliffe, J. H. , Haberman, C. P. , Sorg, E. T. , Joyce, N. M. , & Taylor, R. B. (2015). Does what police do at hot spots matter? The Philadelphia policing tactics experiment . Criminology , 53 ( 1 ), 23–53. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guseynov, N. R. (2010). Policing serious crime: A longitudinal examination of geographically focused policing activities (Master's thesis). Kansas City, MO: University of Missouri‐Kansas City.
  • Hampshire Constabulary . (2006). Operation Mullion: Reducing anti‐social behaviour and crime in and around Mayfield School . Portsmouth, Hamphshire: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hollywood Police Department . (2015). West district burglary reduction initiative . Hollywood, FL: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Houston Police Department . (2012). Back from the brink: Reclaiming the Antoine corridor and the development of problem oriented policing within the Houston Police Department . Houston, TX: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knoxville Police Department . (2002). The Knoxville public safety collaborative . Knoxville, TN: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kochel, T. R. , Burruss, G. , & Weisburd, D. (2015). St Louis County Hot Spots in Residential Areas (SCHIRA) final report: Assessing the effects of hot spots policing strategies on police legitimacy, crime, and collective efficacy . Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kochel, T. R. , & Weisburd, D. (2017). Assessing community consequences of implementing hot spots policing in residential areas: findings from a randomized field trial . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 13 ( 2 ), 143–170. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kochel, T. R. , & Weisburd, D. (2019). The impact of hot spots policing on collective efficacy: Findings from a randomized field trial . Justice Quarterly , 36 ( 5 ), 900–928. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lancashire Constabulary . (2008). “Moppin” up dodge . Lancashire: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lancashire Constabulary . (2010). Smashing time – or not? Lancashire: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lancashire Constabulary . (2012). The custody experience: Reducing 1st time entrants into the criminal justice system . Lancashire: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lexington Division of Police . (2009). Community law enforcement action and response program . Lexington, KY: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • London Borough of Enfield . (2011). Safe as houses‐ domestic burglary project . London: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mazerolle, L. , Price, J. F. , & Roehl, J. (2000). Civil remedies and drug control: A randomized field trial in Oakland, California . Evaluation Review , 24 ( 2 ), 212–241. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Niagara County Sheriff's Office . (2011). Operation panther pride . Lockport, NY: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Northhamptonshire Police . (2008). Northampton Countywide Traveler Unit . Northhamptonshire, UK: Tilley Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nunn, S. , Quinet, K. , Rowe, K. , & Christ, D. (2006). Interdiction day: Covert surveillance operations, drugs, and serious crime in an inner‐city neighborhood . Police Quarterly , 9 ( 1 ), 73–99. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Palm Beach County Sheriff's Office . (2012). Smart Policing Initiative: Increasing police legitimacy and reducing victimization against immigrants in Lake Worth . Lake Worth, FL: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Portland Police Bureau . (2018). Zombie houses: The Portland approach to vacant homes . Portland, OR: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ratcliffe, J. H. , Groff, E. R. , Sorg, E. T. , & Haberman, C. P. (2015). Citizens’ reactions to hot spots policing: impacts on perceptions of crime, disorder, safety and police . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 11 ( 3 ), 393–417. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Regina Police Services . (2007). Regina auto theft strategy . Saskatchewan, Canada: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reno Police Department . (2006). Motel interdiction program: A criminal and nuisance abatement project . Reno, NV: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • San Angelo Police Department . (2006). “ See! It's me!” identity theft prevention program . San Angelo, TX.
  • Sherman, L. , Buerger, M. , & Gartin, P. (1989). Repeat call address policing: The Minneapolis RECAP Experiment . Washington, DC: Crime Control Institute. [ Google Scholar ]
  • South Yorkshire Police . (2006). Focusing on car crime: An initiative by South Yorkshire Police to tackle the problem of offenders stealing from Ford Focus cars . Barnsley, South Yorkshire, UK: Tilley Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • State College Police Department . (2011). Reducing crime and disorder in rental properties: An evaluation of the state college nuisance property ordinance . State College, PA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stokes, R. , Donahue, N. , Caron, D. , & Greene, J. R. (1996). Safe Travel to and from School: A Problem‐Oriented Policing Approach . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stone, S. S. (1993). Problem‐Oriented Policing Approach to Drug Enforcement: Atlanta as a Case Study . Emory University. (Ph.D. dissertation). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sussex Police . (2008). Operation athlete . Sussex, UK: Tilley Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor, B. , Koper, C. S. , & Woods, D. J. (2011). A randomized controlled trial of different policing strategies at hot spots of violent crime . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 7 ( 2 ), 149–181. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thomas, G. R. (1998). Coordinated agency network . San Diego, CA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tuffin, R. , Morris, J. , & Poole, A. (2006). An evaluation of the impact of the National Reassurance Policing Programme . London: Home Office Research. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vancouver Police Department . (2009). Reclaiming the ‘street of shame’: A problem oriented solution to Vancouver's entertainment district . Vancouver, BC: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warwickshire Police . (2009). Trolley safe: A design based problem solving response to reduce purse thefts from shoppers in supermarkets . Warwickshire: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilson, D. (in progress). The Relative Incident Rate Ratio Effect Size for Count-Based Impact Evaluations: When an Odds Ratio is Not an Odds Ratio . Working Paper.
  • Wolfe, S. E. , Rojek, J. , Kaminski, R. , & Nix, J. (2015). City of Columbia (SC) Police Department Smart Policing Initiative: Final Report . Retrieved from http://strategiesforpolicinginnovation.com/sites/default/files/2015_Wolfe%20et%20al_SPI_Final%20Report_Submission%20to%20CNA%20and%20BJA.pdf
  • Weisburd, D. , & Green, L. (1995). Policing drug hot spots: The Jersey City drug market analysis experiment . Justice Quarterly , 12 ( 4 ), 711–735. [ Google Scholar ]
  • White, M. D. , & Katz, C. M. (2013). Policing convenience store crime: Lessons from the Glendale, Arizona Smart Policing Initiative . Police Quarterly , 16 ( 3 ), 305–322. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zidar, M. S. , Shafer, J. G. , & Eck, J. E. (2017). Reframing an obvious police problem: Discovery, analysis and response to a manufactured problem in a small city . Policing: A Journal of Policy and Practice , 12 ( 3 ), 316–331. [ Google Scholar ]

REFERENCES TO EXCLUDED STUDIES

  • Anaheim Police Department . (2010). Truancy…ditching a life of crime . Anaheim, CA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arias, E. D. , & Ungar, M. (2009). Community policing and Latin America's citizen security crisis . Comparative Politics , 41 ( 4 ), 409–429. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bässmann, J. (2011). Vehicle theft reduction in Germany: The long‐term effectiveness of electronic immobilization . European Journal on Criminal Policy and Research , 17 ( 3 ), 221–246. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. (2008). Pulling levers focused deterrence strategies and the prevention of gun homicide . Journal of Criminal Justice , 36 , 332–343. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , & Dusseault, D. (2018). Can homicide detectives improve homicide clearance rates? Crime & Delinquency , 64 ( 3 ), 283–315. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , Pierce, G. L. , McDevitt, J. , Bond, B. J. , & Cronin, S. (2008). The strategic prevention of gun violence among gang‐involved offenders . Justice Quarterly , 24 ( 1 ), 132–162. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cincinnati Police Department . (2011). Saving lives with traffic safety . Cincinnati, OH: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corsaro, N. , Brunson, R. K. , & McGarrell, E. F. (2009). Problem‐oriented policing and open‐air drug markets: Examining the Rockford pulling levers deterrence strategy . Crime & Delinquency , 59 ( 7 ), 1085–1107. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corsaro, N. , & McGarrell, E. F. (2010). Reducing homicide risk in Indianapolis between 1997 and 2000 . Journal of Urban Health , 87 ( 5 ), 851–864. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fagan, J. A. , Davies, G. , & Holland, J. (2005). The bustle of horses on a ship: Drug control in New York City public housing (Columbia Public Law & Legal Theory Working Papers. Paper 0589).
  • Fell, J. C. , Fisher, D. A. , Yao, J. , & Scott McKnight, A. (2017). Evaluation of a responsible beverage service and enforcement program: Effects on bar patron intoxication and potential impaired driving by young adults . Traffic injury prevention , 18 ( 6 ), 557–565. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Florence, C. , Shepherd, J. , Brennan, I. , & Simon, T. (2011). Effectiveness of anonymised information sharing and use in health service, police, and local government partnership for preventing violence related injury: experimental study and time series analysis . BMJ , 342 , d3313. 10.1136/bmj.d3313 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hampshire Constabulary . (2007). Operation Kensington: Bringing the business community and the police together . Portsmouth, Hampshire: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higginson, A. & Neville, R. (2014). SysReview [systematic review management software] . Brisbane, Australia: University of Queensland. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hyunseok, J. , Hoover, L. T. , & Joo, H. (2010). An evaluation of Compstat's effect on crime: The Fort Worth experience . Police Quarterly , 13 ( 4 ), 387–412. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koper, C. S. , Woods, D. J. , & Isom, D. (2016). Evaluating a police‐led community initiative to reduce gun violence in St. Louis . Police Quarterly , 19 ( 2 ), 115–149. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kumar, T. K. V. (2012). Impact of community policing on public satisfaction and perception of police: findings from India . International Criminal Justice Review , 22 ( 4 ), 397–415. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lancashire Constabulary . (2017). The Lancashire vulnerable callers project . Lancashire: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Las Vegas Metropolitan Police Department . (2008). The Fremont Corridor initiative: Crime camera and directed patrol impact . Las Vegas, NV: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • LaVigne, N. G. , Owens, C. , & Hetrick, S. S. (2009). Evaluation of Target's safe city initiative: Implementing public‐private partnerships to address crime in retail settings . Washington, DC: The Urban Institute. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lisburn Neighbourhood Team . (2009). Kjet . Lisburn: Tilley Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maguire, E. R. , Johnson, D. , Kuhns, J. B. , & Apostolos, R. (2019). The effects of community policing on fear of crime and perceived safety: findings from a pilot project in Trinidad and Tobago . Policing and Society: An International Journal of Research and Policy , 29 ( 5 ), 491–510. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martinez, N. N. (2013). Team policing revisited: A quasi‐experimental valuation in Las Vegas, Nevada . (Ph.D. dissertation). University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV.
  • Mazerolle, L. , Rombouts, S. , & McBroom, J. (2006). The impact of operational performance reviews on reported crime in Queensland . Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mazerolle, L. , McBroom, J. , & McBroom, J. (2011). Compstat in Australia: An analysis of the spatial and temporal impact . Journal of criminal justice , 39 ( 2 ), 128–136. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mazerolle, L. , Darroch, S. , & White, G. (2013). Leadership in problem‐oriented policing . Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies & Management , 36 ( 3 ), 543–560. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McGarrell, E. F. , Chermak, S. , Wilson, J. M. , & Corsaro, N. (2006). Reducing homicide through a “lever‐pulling” strategy . Justice Quarterly , 23 ( 2 ), 214–231. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McGarrell, E. F. , Corsaro, N. , Hipple, N. K. , & Bynum, T. S. (2010). Project Safe Neighborhoods and violent crime trends in US cities: Assessing violent crime impact . Journal of Quantitative Criminology , 26 ( 2 ), 165–190. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Metropolitan Police Service . (2006). Preventing disorder, pr omoting diversion and protecting diversity: Working together to create safer and cleaner communities on the Pembury Estate . London: Tilley Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Neville, R. & Higginson, A. (2014). Optimal Search of Indexing Databases (OSID) (Computer software) . Brisbane, Australia: University of Queensland. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norman Police Department . (2012). My body…my life…Empowering women through awareness, education, violence prevention and self‐defense techniques . Norman, OK: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Providence Police Department . (2018). Reducing street level prostitution: Community partnerships and intervention . Providence, RI: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rajaee, L. , Madden Rodriguez, J. , Addison, S. , Readio, S. , & Longwood, W. (2013). Law enforcement advocate program: An interagency collaboration to enhance community‐police relations and improve outcomes for substance‐using offenders . Juvenile and Family Court Journal , 64 ( 2 ), 17–33. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sedelmaier, C. M. , & Hipple, N. K. (2016). New Haven, Connecticut Smart Policing Initiative: Employing evidence‐based policing strategies to engage the community and reduce crime . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Assistance. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sullivan, W. J. (2013). Kentucky SRO programs: An examination of impact on reported criminal violations and board violations (Ph.D. dissertation). Richmond, KY: Eastern Kentucky University.
  • Sussex Police . (2008). Operation athlete . Sussex: Tilley Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wagers, M. L. (2007). Broken windows policing: The LAPD experience (Ph.D. dissertation). Rutgers, NJ: Rutgers University.
  • Washington State Patrol . (2006). Ticketing aggressive cars and trucks (TACT) . Olympia, WA: Herman Goldstein Award Submission. [ Google Scholar ]

ADDITIONAL REFERENCES

  • Bowers, K. , Johnson, S. , Guerette, R. T. , Summers, L. , & Poynton, S. (2011). Spatial displacement and diffusion of benefits among geographically focused policing interventions: A meta‐analytical review . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 7 ( 4 ), 347–374. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blattman, C. , Green, D. , Ortega, D. , & Tobón, S. (2017). Pushing crime around the corner? Estimating experimental impacts of large‐scale security interventions . Unpublished manuscript.
  • Braga, A. A. (2001). The effects of hot spots policing on crime . The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science , 578 ( 1 ), 104–125. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. (2007). Effects of hot spots policing on crime. A Campbell Collaboration systematic review . Campbell Systematic Reviews , 7 ( 1 ), 1–39. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. (2008). Problem‐oriented policing and crime prevention (2nd ed.). Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , Kennedy, D. M. , Waring, E. J. , & Piehl, A. M. (2001). Problem‐oriented policing, deterrence, and youth violence: An evaluation of Boston's Operation Ceasefire . Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency , 38 ( 3 ), 195–225. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , Papachristos, A. V. , & Hureau, D. M. (2014). The effects of hot spots policing on crime: An updated systematic review and meta‐analysis . Justice Quarterly , 31 ( 4 ), 633–663. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , Turchan, B. , Papachristos, A. V. , & Hureau, D. M. (2019). Hot spots policing of small geographic areas effects on crime . Campbell Systematic Reviews , 15 ( 3 ), 1–88. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , & Weisburd, D. (2010). Policing problem places: Crime hot spots and effective prevention . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braga, A. A. , Weisburd, D. , & Turchan, B. (2019). Focused deterrence strategies effects on crime: A systematic review, Campbell Systematic Reviews ( 15 , pp. 1–65. 3 . [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell, D. T. , & Stanley, J. (1966). Ex perimental and quasi‐experimental designs for research . Chicago, IL: Rand McNally. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Capowich, G. E. , & Roehl, J. A. (1994). Problem‐oriented policing: Actions and effectiveness in San Diego. In Rosenbaum D. P. (Ed.), The challenge of community policing: Testing the promises (pp. 127–146). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clarke, R. V. (1992a). Situational crime prevention: Theory and practice, British Journal of Criminology ( 20 , pp. 136–147. 2 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clarke, R. V. (1992b). Situational crime prevention: Successful case studies . Albany, NY: Harrow and Heston. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clarke, R. V. (Ed.). (1997). Situational crime prevention: Successful case studies (2nd ed.). Albany, NY: Harrow & Heston. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clarke, R. V. , & Goldstein, H. (2002). Reducing theft at construction sites: Lessons from a problem‐oriented project. In Tilley N. (Ed.), Analysis for crime prevention: Crime prevention studies Volume 13 (pp. 89–130). Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cook, T. , & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi‐experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings . Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cordner, G. W. (1986). Fear of crime and the police: An evaluation of a fear‐reduction strategy . Journal of Police Science and Administration , 14 ( 3 ), 223–233. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cordner, G. W. , & Biebel, E. P. (2005). Problem‐oriented policing in practice . Criminology and Public Policy , 4 ( 2 ), 155–180. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duval, S. J. , & Tweedie, R. (2000). A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta‐analysis . Journal of the American Statistical Association , 95 ( 449 ), 89–98. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eck, J. E. (2002). Preventing crime at places. In Sherman L. W., Farrington D., Welsh B. & MacKenzie D. L. (Eds.), Evidence‐based crime prevention (pp. 241–294). New York: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eck, J. E. (2006a). When is a bologna sandwich better than sex? A defense of small‐n case study evaluations . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 2 ( 3 ), 345–362. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eck, J. E. (2006b). Science, values, and problem‐oriented policing: Why problem‐oriented policing? In Weisburd D. & Braga A. A. (Eds.), Police innovation: Contrasting perspectives (pp. 117–132). Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eck, J. E. , & Spelman, W. (1987). Who ya gonna call? The police as problem‐busters . Crime & Delinquency , 33 ( 1 ), 31–52. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Farrington, D. (2006). Methodological quality and the evaluation of anticrime programs . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 2 ( 3 ), 329–337. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Farrington, D. P. , Gill, M. , Waples, S. J. , & Argomaniz, J. (2007). The effects of closed‐circuit television on crime: Meta‐analysis of an English national quasi‐experimental multi‐site evaluation . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 3 ( 1 ), 21–38. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hasselblad, V. , & Hedges, L. V. (1995). Meta‐analysis of screening and diagnostic tests . Psychological Bulletin , 117 ( 1 ), 167–178. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Glass, G. V. (1997). Interrupted time series quasi‐experiments. In Jaeger R. M. (Ed.), Complementary methods for research in education . 2nd ed. (pp. 589–608). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldstein, H. (1979). Improving policing: A problem‐oriented approach . Crime and Delinquency , 25 ( 2 ), 236–258. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldstein, H. (1990). Problem‐oriented policing . Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldstein, H. (2018). On problem‐oriented policing: The Stockholm lecture . Crime Science , 7 ( 13 ), 1–9. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guerette, R. T. , & Bowers, K. J. (2009). Assessing the extent of crime displacement and diffusion of benefits: A review of situational crime prevention evaluations . Criminology , 47 ( 4 ), 1331–1368. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higginson, A. , Eggins, E. , Mazerolle, L. , & Stanko, E. (2015). The Global Policing Database [Database and Protocol] . Retrieved from http://www.gpd.uq.edu.au
  • Hope, T. (1994). Problem‐oriented policing and drug market locations: Three case studies. In Clarke R. V. (Ed.), Crime prevention studies ( 2 , pp. 5–31). Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kansas City Police Department . (1977). Response time analysis: Executive summary . Kansas City, MO: Board of Commissioners. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kelling, G. L. , Pate, T. , Dieckman, D. , & Brown, C. E. (1974). The Kansas City preventive patrol experiment: Technical report . Washington, DC: Police Foundation. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kennedy, D. M. , Braga, A. A. , Piehl, A. M. , & Waring, E. J. (2001). Reducing gun violence: The Boston Gun Project's Operation Ceasefire . Washington, DC: United States Department of Justice, National Institute of Justice. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lipsey, M. W. (1990). Design sensitivity: Statistical power for experimental research . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lösel, F. (2018). Evidence comes by replication, but needs differentiation: the reproducibility issue in science and its relevance for criminology . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 14 ( 3 ), 257–278. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lösel, F. , & Köferl, P. (1989). Evaluation research on correctional treatment in West Germany: A meta‐analysis. In Wegener H., Lösel F. & Haisch J. (Eds.), Criminal behavior and the justice system (pp. 334–355). New York: Springer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maguire, E. R. , Uchida, C. D. , & Hassell, K. D. (2015). Problem‐oriented policing in Colorado Springs: A content analysis of 753 cases . Crime & Delinquency , 61 ( 1 ), 71–95. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mazerolle, L. G. , Ready, J. , Terrill, W. , & Waring, E. (2000). Problem‐oriented policing in public housing: The Jersey City evaluation . Justice Quarterly , 17 ( 1 ), 129–158. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perry, A. , & Johnson, M. (2008). Applying the consolidated standards of reporting trials (CONSORT) to studies of mental health provision for juvenile offenders: A research note . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 4 ( 2 ), 165–185. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perry, A. , Weisburd, D. , & Hewitt, C. (2010). Are criminologists reporting experiments in ways that allow us to assess them? Journal of Experimental Criminology , 6 ( 3 ), 245–263. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Poyner, B. (1981). Crime prevention and the environment—Street attacks in city centres . Police Research Bulletin , 37 , 10–18. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reaves, B. A. (2015). L ocal polic e de partments, 2013: Personnel, policies, and practices . Washington, DC: United States Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Bureau of Justice Statistics. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reisig, M. D. (2010). Community and problem oriented policing . Crime and Justice , 39 ( 1 ), 1–53. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reppetto, T. (1976). Crime prevention and the displacement phenomenon . Crime & Delinquency , 22 ( 2 ), 166–177. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rothstein, H. R. (2008). Publication bias as a threat to the validity of meta‐analytic results . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 4 ( 1 ), 61–81. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rossi, P. H. (1987). The iron law of evaluation and other metallic rules . Research in Social Problems and Public Policy , 4 ( 1 ), 3–20. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scott, M. S. (2000). Problem‐oriented policing: Reflections on the first 20 years . Washington, DC: United States Department of Justice, Office of Community Oriented Policing Services. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scott, M. S. , & Goldstein, H. (2005). Ron Clarke's contribution to improving policing: A diffusion of benefits. In Farrell G. & Tilley N. (Eds.), The reasoning criminologist: Essays in honour of Ronald V. Clarke (pp. 93–107). London: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scott, M. S. & Clarke, R. V. (Eds.). (2020). Problem‐oriented policing: Successful case studies . New York: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shadish, W. R. , Cook, T. D. , & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi‐experimental designs for generalized causal inference . Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sidebottom, A. , & Tilley, N. (2011). Improving problem‐oriented policing: The need for a new model? Crime Prevention and Community Safety , 13 ( 2 ), 79–101. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Skogan, W. , & Frydl, K. (2004). Fairne ss and effectiveness in policing: The evidence . Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Solé Brito, C. & Allan, T. (Eds.). (1999). Problem‐oriented policing: Crime‐specific problems, critical issues and making POP work ( II ). Washington, DC: Police Executive Research Forum. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spelman, W. , & Eck, J. E. (1987). Problem‐oriented policing . Washington, DC: United States Department of Justice, National Institute of Justice. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Telep, C. W. , Weisburd, D. , Gill, C. E. , Vitter, Z. , & Teichman, D. (2014). Displacement of crime and diffusion of crime control benefits in large‐scale geographic areas: A systematic review . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 10 ( 4 ), 515–548. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. (1997). Reorienting crime prevention research and policy: From the causes of criminality to the context of crime . Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. & Braga, A. A. (Eds.). (2006). Police innovation: Contrasting perspectives . New York: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , & Eck, J. E. (2004). What can the police do to reduce crime, disorder and fear? Annals of the American Academy of Social and Political Sciences , 593 ( 1 ), 42–65. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , Hasisi, B. , Litmanovitz, Y. , Carmel, T. , & Tshuva, S. Overcoming the “means over ends syndrome” in policing: An evaluation of the EMUN reform in Israel . Unpublished manuscript.
  • Weisburd, D. , Lum, C. M. , & Yang, S. (2003). When can we conclude that treatments or programs “don't work”? The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science , 587 ( 1 ), 31–48. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , & Majmundar, D. K. (2018). Proactive policing: effects on crime and communities . Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , & Telep, C. W. (2014). Hot spots policing: What we know and what we need to know . Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice , 30 ( 2 ), 200–220. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , Telep, C. W. , Hinkle, J. C. , & Eck, J. E. (2008). The effects of problem‐oriented policing on crime and disorder . Campbell Systematic Reviews , 4 ( 1 ), 1–87. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , Telep, C. W. , Hinkle, J. C. , & Eck, J. E. (2010). Is problem‐oriented policing effective in reducing crime and disorder? Findings from a Campbell systematic review . Criminology & Public Policy , 9 ( 1 ), 139–172. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , Willis, J. J. , Mastrofski, S. D. , & Greenspan, R. (2019). Changing everything so that everything can remain the same: CompStat and American policing. In Weisburd David & Braga Anthony A. (Eds.), Police innovation: Contrasting perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 417–438). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisburd, D. , Wyckoff, L. A. , Ready, J. , Eck, J. E. , Hinkle, J. C. , & Gajewski, F. (2006). Does crime just move around the corner? A controlled study of spatial displacement and diffusion of crime control benefits . Criminology , 44 ( 3 ), 549–592. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Welsh, B. C. (2016). Making sense of crime prevention evaluation research and communicating it for the public good . Criminology & Public Policy , 15 ( 3 ), 611–615. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilson, D. B. (2009). Missing a critical piece of the pie: Simple document search strategies inadequate for systematic reviews . Journal of Experimental Criminology , 5 ( 4 ), 429–440. [ Google Scholar ]

Criminal Justice Know How

Criminal Justice Know How

We connect community with the criminal justice system.

  • Law Enforcement

The S.A.R.A. Model

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

by Kelly M. Glenn, 2020

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

When we prevent crime, we prevent victimization, which is the ultimate goal! Several theories exist involving crime prevention, including (but not limited to):

  • Crime Prevention Through Environmental Design (C.P.T.E.D.)
  • The Broken Window Theory
  • The S.A.R.A. Model (Scan, Analyze, Respond, Assess)
  • The Crime Prevention Triangle

The S.A.R.A. Model of crime prevention is a part of what was coined as “Problem-Oriented Policing” by Herman Goldstein in 1979.  Problem-Oriented Policing, or POP, was a response to reactive, incident-driven policing in which successes in addressing community problems were short-lived.  Before we get into how the S.A.R.A. Model changed that, let’s take a look at an example of a law enforcement response that would be considered reactive and incident-driven with short-lived success:

Officer Comar was assigned to patrol a densely populated downtown area where foot traffic was fairly moderate.  Maris, owner of a local convenience store, called 9-11 nearly every day wanting the police department to come run off the drunks that would come into her store, buy a beer, and then hang around on her sidewalk drinking and asking other patrons to buy them more alcohol.  For Maris, her store felt more like a get together for middle-aged men who traded showers, shaving, and jobs for getting drunk on her doorstep by 10 a.m. Once the store closed for the night, she would spend considerable time waking up the ones that had passed out and cleaning up their trash.  Her main complaint, though, was that they alienated customers and that she was losing good business because of them. Going to the store and running them off was part of Officer Comar’s daily routine. In fact, because she went so often, she got into the habit of pulling into the parking lot, even if Maris didn’t call, hitting her siren, and watching them disperse.  Officer Comar considered this good, proactive police work, and Maris was happy that she didn’t always have to pick up the phone to solve the problem.  

In our scenario, we can clearly identify a community problem:  the local convenience store was overrun by alcohol addicts, and law-abiding citizens were avoiding the business to avoid the drunks.  But was Officer Comar truly doing good, proactive police work by showing up several times a shift to run them off, and was Maris getting the best service from her local police department?  

We’ll find out!

In 1987, Eck and Spelman built upon the Problem-Oriented Policing approach by using the S.A.R.A. Model to address community problems and crime.  S.A.R.A. looks to identify and overcome the underlying causes of crime and disorder versus just treating the symptoms.  It can be applied to any community problem by implementing each of four steps in the model:  Scanning, Analysis, Response, and Assessment.

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

First Step – Scanning

During the scanning phase, law enforcement works with community members to identify existing or potential problems and prioritize them.  It’s helpful to answer a few questions within this phase:

  • Is this problem real or perceived?   For example, do the 291 calls for service to report speeders in a residential area really mean that drivers are exceeding the speed limit?  Or, are the calls coming from one resident who is irritated that drivers won’t slow down to below the speed limit when she crosses the street to check her mail?
  • What are the consequences of not addressing this problem?   Are the consequences merely a matter of inconvenience for some people, or does this particular problem impact the health, safety, prosperity, happiness, etc. of community members?
  • How often does this problem occur?   Is it daily?  Weekly? Just during certain seasons, or when a big event occurs in town?

If we think about Maris and her convenience store, we can clearly identify a few existing symptoms of a problem.  First, Maris is losing business due to the drunks hanging out on her sidewalk all day. Second, other community members who may rely on this convenience store as their easiest option for groceries and goods, may be avoiding it to avoid the drunks.  Third, Maris is maintaining somewhat of a common nuisance. She has an environment that is conducive to crime and disorder, which is creating a burden on local police services. Prioritizing this community problem and reducing or eliminating the aforementioned symptoms by tackling the root cause(s) could be a win for many people.  Let answer the above questions with our scenario in mind:

  • Is this problem real or perceived?   The problem is real.  Maris’s declining sales, the police department’s calls for service, and Officer Comar’s own observations and actions support the legitimacy of the issue.
  • What are the consequences of not addressing this problem?   This problem will not go away on its own.  In fact, if trouble loves company, we can predict that the group of drunks will continue to grow, thus increasing the calls for service to the police department.  Additionally, Maris’s business will continue to struggle, and without enough customers, the convenience store could eventually close, creating a burden for citizens who do depend upon it.  
  • How often does this problem occur?   As calls for service show, this problem is a daily occurance.  More than likely, it is a bigger problem during fair weather than when it’s cold or rainy; however, the problem is consistent and reocurring.

Now that we’ve scanned for the community problem, identified it, and prioritized it by answering some questions, let’s tackle our next step!

Second Step:  Analysis

When we analyze a known community problem, we use relevant data to learn more.  Our goal is to be effective in reducing or eliminating it, so we must pinpoint possible explanations for why or how the problem is occuring.  Again, we can ask some useful questions to guide us:

  • What relevant data is available?   Statistics?  Calls for service?  Demographics?
  • What are some possible explanations for why or how the problem is occuring?   Are there environmental issues?  Is there a behavioral issue? Is there a lack of appropriate legislation or policy to enforce a solution?  Is there a lack of community services?  
  • What is currently being done to address the problem? Is anything at all being done? If something is being done, why is it ineffective? Who is involved in the current response? What resources are being dedicated to the current response? 

Let’s take a look at how we can answer these questions when working with Maris within our scenario:

  • What relevant data is available?   Maris can provide records for declining sales, and they can be compared to various seasons of the year when weather may impact the gatherings of the local drunks outside of her convenience store.  The police department can use the number of calls for service, as well as data on how each call for service was cleared (arrest, warning, report, etc.). The police department can also see if other more serious crimes are linked to this problem (physical fights between drunks, thefts out of customer vehicles, etc.).  Collectively, they can identify the average ages of the individuals, as well as their socioeconomic status.
  • Maris’s store is open to the public, and the drunks are part of the public.  
  • Maris’s store is located in an area that is accessible to foot traffic, and these drunks live in nearby housing. 
  • These drunks suffer from an addiction to alcohol, and Maris sells beer.  
  • The drunks can pay for the beer whether it be from money they earn, money they receive in public assistance, or money that is given to them by other generous customers.
  • Maris calls the police department when she wants the drunks to leave; although sometimes, Officer Comar will automatically address the issue when she drives by.
  • The current response is ineffective because the drunks come back later and/or return the following day.
  • Maris, Officer Comar, and the local police department are involved in the current response.
  • The current response depletes the taxpayer funded resources via the use of the local police department.

Now that a lot of the brain work is done, it’s time to turn ideas into action.  Let’s take a look at the third step in the S.A.R.A. Model:

Third Step:  Response

In this phase of addressing crime, law enforcement and community partners work together to identify and select responses, or interventions, that are most likely to lead to long-term success in reducing or eliminating the community problem they have scanned and analyzed.

Two questions should be asked during this phase:

  • What are some possible ways to address the problem?  Do we need more community partners?  Do we need to alter access? Do we need to install monitoring devices?  Does a law or policy need to be implemented or changed? Do we need to better enforce the ones we already have?  Do we need to make a list of community services and make referrals? 
  • Which of the potential responses are going to be most successful? Which interventions will attack the root causes, not the symptoms? What interventions will have a positive long-term impact?

Using our scenario, let’s list some possible ways to address Maris’s problem at her convenience store, as well as select the interventions that are likely going to lead to long-term success. Remember, this is a team effort, and Maris definitely should have some input!

  • Although Maris’s store is open to the public, her business is privately owned and located on private property.  Existing laws in her locality protect private business and property owners by allowing them to bar people from the property as long as it is not discriminatory based on protected classes, so Maris does have the legal authority to ban the drunks from her property and business.  During a meeting with the police department, in which everyone is sharing information and working together to come up with a response plan, Officer Comar confirms what Maris already suspected:  many of these addicts have long histories of arrests for public intoxication, trespassing, etc., and going to jail for a night or two isn’t much of a deterrent for them. While she can go through the effort of barring each one and the police department can make arrests, both she and the police department agree that it’s not the most effective route for long-term success.  This intervention was eliminated from the response plan.
  • Maris’s store is accessible to foot traffic, which is both a blessing and a curse.  There is nothing she can do or wants to do to alter the way her customers enter her business or property.  With that said, Maris does not have “No Loitering” signs posted on the property, and her locality has enforceable loitering laws.  “No Loitering” signage could motivate customers to make their necessary transactions and leave, but she has always been hesitant to put them up because she does not want to appear “unfriendly” to youth who come by and chat over a bag of chips and a soda.  She also learns that despite there being a local ordinance against loitering, the local judges are hesitant to impose any significant sanctions for it. Maris opts not to install “No Loitering” signage. This intervention was eliminated from the response plan.
  • Maris offers a product (beer) that her problem customers are addicted to, but it’s also a popular product among her good customers.  Regardless, she could stop selling it. Upon some discussion, Maris shares that she is unwilling to stop selling beer.  It is one of her best selling products, and she has a loyal customer base who have kept her in business by overlooking and literally overstepping the drunks to come in and buy their favorite case of beer from her.  This prompted Officer Comar to ask, “Are your best customers the ones who come in, buy their beer, and leave with beer as singles out of the cooler or as warm cases of beer you have stocked on the shelves?” Maris thought about it quickly and responded that the warm cases of beer are cheaper.  Her customers often opt for those and simply put their beer in their fridge when they get home. As if a light came on, the entire group began talking about how her problem customers are not going to be interested in drinking warm beer as soon as they leave the store. If Maris were to stop selling single beers in the cooler, the group of drunks may stop gathering on her sidewalk all day long. Maris was very open to trying this. 
  • During the meeting, the police department also advises Maris that she does have the legal authority to refuse service to anyone, even paying customers. Technically, she can refuse to serve the drunks.  Maris gave it some thought, but instead of refusing service to the group of drunks that frequent her store, she decides that she will continue to sell them warm cases of beer or any other product they opt to purchase.  After all, many of them also buy snacks or may have limited access to food. This intervention of refusing them service altogether was eliminated from the response plan.  
  • Finally, the police department and Maris acknowledge that while they don’t have control over everything, they can’t ignore the fact that addiction plays a role in this community problem. Officer Comar shares that she has access to a list of free resources for addicts, including locations and times of AA Meetings.  The police department could make a poster with the information, and Maris could hang it in the window of her store. Maris liked this idea. It made her feel as if she was contributing to a solution for a problem that is bigger than just her and her convenience store.

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

As we can see, out of all of that scanning and analyzing, only two responses appear to be a viable long-term strategy, but that’s okay!  Even one effective strategy is better than a dozen ineffective ones. 

Now that we have two good possibilities, let’s look at what needs to be included in our response plan:

  • an outline of each potential response, 
  • the objective for each potential response, 
  • who is implementing each response, and 
  • the responsibilities of each person or agency implementing each response.

Let’s finish out our plan of action for the interventions Maris, Officer Comar, and the police department agreed upon.

RESPONSE PLAN – Intervention #1

  • Outline:  Maris will stop selling individual beers in the cooler at her convenience store for six months but will continue selling warm cases of beer on the shelves. 
  • Objective:  To eliminate the desire of drunk patrons to loiter at Maris’s convenience store
  • Who Implements:  Maris
  • Maris will contact her distributors and alter her beer orders for six months.
  • Maris will instruct her employees not to stock individual beers in the cooler for six months.
  • Maris will post signage on the cooler doors that state, “We no longer sell individual beers.  Please select from our great variety of cases on Aisle 2.”

RESPONSE PLAN – Intervention #2

  • Outline: Maris will also hang a poster about local AA Meetings and resources for addicts in the window of her convenience store.
  • Objective: to provide referrals to resources available for addicts
  • Who Implements: Office Comar and Maris
  • Officer Comar will compile all of the AA Meeting locations, dates, and times and will create a poster.
  • Maris will hang the AA Meeting poster in the window of her store.

Now, we sit back and wait!  The team agreed to carry out the last step of the S.A.R.A. Model, Asssessment, in six months.  Let’s take a look at what that will entail.

Fourth Step – Assessment

During the final phase of the S.A.R.A. Model, the team evaluates two questions:

  • Was each intervention in the response plan implemented in a way that was consistent with the plan?   Did each person and/or agency carry out their responsibilities?  Did anyone veer off course? Did the plan lose momentum?  
  • Did the response achieve their intended effects?   Were the objectives met?  If not, why?

When the team from our scenario met again six months later, they assessed their response by answering both questions:

  • Was each response implemented in a way that was consistent with the plan? It was determined that Maris, Office Comar, and the police department stuck to the plan, and each person/agency followed through on their responsibilities for the entire six months.
  • Did the response plan achieve their intended effects?   Yes!  Maris described the first few days as a little rough.  In fact, she had to call the police department more than once to handle customers who became disorderly when they saw that she was no longer offering single beers in the cooler.  Officer Comar had been the one to show up on a few occasions, but both were happy to report that once they made it through the first week, it was smooth sailing. The kind of customers who are currently frequenting the store are the kind of customers Maris wants, and she’s even seen her sales pick up.  As for assessing whether or not addicts received referrals to community resources through the posters Officer Comar made and Maris hung, it’s difficult to ascertain. While Officer Comar did call around to the AA Meeting hosts to find out if any of the known addicts from the convenience store had attended meetings, AA explained that their meetings and attendees must remain confidential.  

Of course, not every response is going to be as successful as the one in our scenario, but in those cases, teams can demonstrate their commitment to Problem-Oriented Policing by revisiting the S.A.R.A. Model and determining which steps need to be repeated.  

One pitfall should also be noted, even with the most successful responses:  As with any success, we can sometimes become complacent, and when we become complacent, we let down our guard.  This is when small cracks in the response occur, and the problem can get a foothold again. For example, in our scenario, if Maris feels like things are going so well that she can probably start selling single beers in the cooler again, the problem behaviors could eventually return.  It’s never a bad idea to schedule follow-up assessments just to ensure the response is still working!

Finally, it’s important to highlight again that the S.A.R.A. Model cannot be successful without the involvement of community partners.  In our scenario, it took collaboration at each stage to reach a long-term solution that worked.

Many thanks to two former students, Miller Comar and Maris Benar, and former law enforcement officer John Moisa, for serving as inspiration for the S.A.R.A. Model scenario!

Suggested Citation for this Article

Glenn, K.M., Criminal Justice Know How, LLC, 2020,  The S.A.R.A. Model , https://criminaljusticeknowhow.com/the-sara-model/

Related Posts

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

National Law Enforcement Appreciation Day

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

5 Things Every Domestic Violence Report Should Include

what is a strength of the sara problem solving process

Thank You to Our 911 Dispatchers

2015 POP Conference Oct 19-21, 2015 Portland, OR

Center for Problem Oriented Policing

Center for Problem-Oriented Policing

Powerd by University at Albany, SUNY

  • » What is Problem-Oriented Policing?
  • » History of Problem-Oriented Policing
  • » Key Elements of POP
  • « The SARA Model
  • » The Problem Analysis Triangle
  • » Situational Crime Prevention
  • » 25 Techniques
  • » Links to Other POP Friendly Sites
  • » About POP en Español

The SARA Model

A commonly used problem-solving method is the SARA model (Scanning, Analysis, Response and Assessment). The SARA model contains the following elements:

  • Identifying recurring problems of concern to the public and the police.
  • Identifying the consequences of the problem for the community and the police.
  • Prioritizing those problems.
  • Developing broad goals.
  • Confirming that the problems exist.
  • Determining how frequently the problem occurs and how long it has been taking place.
  • Selecting problems for closer examination.
  • Identifying and understanding the events and conditions that precede and accompany the problem.
  • Identifying relevant data to be collected.
  • Researching what is known about the problem type.
  • Taking inventory of how the problem is currently addressed and the strengths and limitations of the current response.
  • Narrowing the scope of the problem as specifically as possible.
  • Identifying a variety of resources that may be of assistance in developing a deeper understanding of the problem.
  • Developing a working hypothesis about why the problem is occurring.
  • Brainstorming for new interventions.
  • Searching for what other communities with similar problems have done.
  • Choosing among the alternative interventions.
  • Outlining a response plan and identifying responsible parties.
  • Stating the specific objectives for the response plan.
  • Carrying out the planned activities.

Assessment:

  • Determining whether the plan was implemented (a process evaluation).
  • Collecting pre– and post–response qualitative and quantitative data.
  • Determining whether broad goals and specific objectives were attained.
  • Identifying any new strategies needed to augment the original plan.
  • Conducting ongoing assessment to ensure continued effectiveness.
  • Translations
  • Privacy Policy
  • POP Projects
  • Learning Center
  • POP Conference

Copyright © 2016 Center for Problem-Oriented Policing

WACOP

  • 2023 WACOP Board Members
  • Conferences
  • Community Policing Resources

The SARA Problem Solving Model

The SARA Model Approach to Problem Solving Problem-solving is an integral component of the philosophy of community policing. The problem-solving approach is a methodical process for reducing the impact of crime and disorder problems in a community. One problem solving model frequently used is the SARA model (Scanning, Analysis, Response, Assessment). Defined below, this four-step process is implemented by the policing agency in partnership with the community.

Scanning The identification of a cluster of similar, related or recurring incidents through a preliminary review of information, and the selection of this crime/disorder problem, among competing priorities, for future examination.

Analysis The use of several sources of information to determine why a problem is occurring, who is responsible, who is affected, where the problem is located, when it occurs, and what form the problem takes. Analysis requires identifying patterns that explain the conditions that facilitate the crime or disorder problem. Sources of information may include police data (CAD, arrest, incident data, etc.), victim and offender interviews; environmental surveys; officer, business and resident surveys; social service and other government agency data; insurance information, etc.

Response The execution of a tailored set of actions that address the most important findings of the problem analysis phase and focus on at least two of the following: (1) preventing future occurrences by deflecting offenders; (2) protecting likely victims; or (3) making crime locations less conducive to problem behaviors. Responses are designed to have a long-term impact on the problem, and do not require a commitment of police time and resources that is not sustainable over the long-term.

Assessment The measurement of the impact(s) of the responses on the targeted crime/disorder problem using information collected from multiple sources, both before and after the responses have been implemented.

www.usdoj.gov/cops/cp_resources/cp_sara.htm

U.S. Department of Justice

Office of Community Oriented Policing Services

1100 Vermont Avenue, NW, Washington, DC 20530

(202) 514-2058

DOJ Response Center: 1 (800) 421-6770

Wisconsin Association of Community Oriented Police Themeshopy

2015 POP Conference Oct 19-21, 2015 Portland, OR

Center for Problem Oriented Policing

Center for Problem-Oriented Policing

Powerd by University at Albany, SUNY

  • Print This Guide
  • Order Bound Copy
  • Download PDF Guide
  • Share This Guide

Introduction

Conducting Impact Evaluations

Evaluation Designs

Appendix A: The Effects of the Number of Time Periods on the Validity of Evaluation Conclusions

Appendix B: Evaluation Designs With Control Groups

Appendix C: Calculating a Response's Net Effect

Appendix D: Summary Of Evaluation Designs' Strengths and Weaknesses

Appendix E: Problem-Solving Evaluation Checklist

Assessing Responses to Problems: An Introductory Guide for Police Problem-Solvers

Tool guide no. 1 (2002).

by John E. Eck

The purpose of assessing a problem-solving effort is to help you make better decisions by answering two specific questions. First, did the problem decline? Answering this question helps you decide whether to end the problem-solving effort and focus resources on other problems. Second, if the problem did decline, did the response cause the decline? Answering this question helps you decide whether to apply the response to similar problems.

What This Guide Is About

This introduction to problem-solving assessments is intended to help you design evaluations to answer the two questions above. It was written for those who are responsible for evaluating the effectiveness of responses to problems, and who have a basic understanding of problem-oriented policing and the problem-solving process. This guide assumes a basic understanding of the SARA problem-solving process (scanning, analysis, response, and assessment), but it requires little or no experience with assessing problem solutions.

This guide was written based on the assumption that you have no outside assistance. Nevertheless, you should seek the advice and help of researchers with training and experience in evaluation, particularly if the problem you are addressing is large and complex. Requesting aid from an independent outside evaluator can be particularly helpful if there is controversy over a response's usefulness. Local colleges and universities are a good source for such expertise. Many social science departments–economics, political science, sociology, psychology, and criminal justice/criminology–have faculty and graduate students who are knowledgeable in program evaluation and related topics.

This guide is a companion reference to the Problem-Oriented Guides for Police series. Each guide in the series suggests ways to measure a particular problem, and describes possible responses to it. Though the evaluation principles discussed here are intended to apply to the specific problems in the guides, you should be able to apply them to any problemsolving project.

This is an introduction to a complex subject, and it emphasizes evaluation methods that are the most relevant to problem-oriented policing. † You should consult the list of recommended readings at the end of the guide if you are interested in exploring the topic of evaluation in greater detail.

† Excluded from this discussion is any mention of significance testing and statistical estimation. Though useful methods, they cannot be described in a guide of this length sufficiently enough for you to effectively use them.

Assessment and Decision-Making

As stated, this guide is about aiding decision–making. There are two key decisions to make regarding any problem-solving effort. First, did the problem decline enough for you to end the effort and apply resources elsewhere? If the problem did not decline substantially, then the job is not done. In such a case, the most appropriate decision may be to reanalyze the problem and develop a new response. Second, if the problem did decline substantially, then it might be worthwhile to apply the response to similar problems.

This guide focuses on the first decision–whether to end the problem-solving effort. The second decision has to do with future response applications. If the problem declined substantially, and if the response at least partly caused the decline, then you might consider using the response with other problems. But if the problem did not decline, or if it got worse, and this was due to an ineffective response, then future problem-solvers should be alerted so they can develop better responses to similar problems. Future decisions about whether to use the response depend in part on assessment information. In this regard, assessment is an essential part of police organizational learning. Without assessments, problemsolvers are constantly reinventing the wheel, and run the risk of repeating the same mistakes. Nevertheless, obtaining valid information to aid in decision-making increases the complexity of assessments.

Making either decision requires a detailed understanding of the problem, of how the response is supposed to reduce the problem, and of the context in which the response has been implemented. 1 For this reason, the evaluation process begins after it is identified in the scanning stage.

This guide discusses two simple designs–pre-post and interrupted time series. The pre-post design is useful in making only the first type of decision–whether to end the problem-solving effort. The time series design can aid in making both types of decisions.

Finally, it is worth mentioning how the guide is organized. The body of the text addresses fundamental issues in constructing simple but useful evaluations. The endnotes provide a link to more-technical books on evaluation. Many of these clarify terminology. The appendixes expand on material in the text. Appendix A uses an extended example to show why evaluating responses over longer periods provides a better understanding of response effectiveness. Appendix B describes two advanced designs involving comparison (or "control" groups). Appendix C explains how to calculate a response's net effect on a problem. Appendix D provides a summary of the designs' strengths and weaknesses. Finally, Appendix E provides a checklist for going through the evaluation process, selecting the most applicable design, and drawing reasonable conclusions from evaluation results. You should read the body of the text before examining the appendixes.

In summary, this guide explains, in ordinary language, those aspects of evaluation methods that are most important to police when addressing problems. In the next section, we will examine how evaluation fits within the SARA problemsolving process. We will then examine the two major types of evaluation–process and impact.

Evaluation’s Role in Problem-Solving

It is important to distinguish between evaluation and assessment. Evaluation is scientific process for determining if a problem declined and if the solution caused the decline. As we will see, it begins at the moment the problem-solving process begins and continues through the completion of the effort. Assessment occurs at the final stage in the SARA problem-solving process. 2 It is the culmination of the evaluation process, the time when you draw conclusions about the problem and its solutions.

Though assessment is the final stage of both evaluation and problem solving, critical decisions about the evaluation are made throughout the process, as indicated in Figure 1. The left side shows the standard SARA process and some of the most basic questions asked at each stage. It also draws attention to the fact that the assessment may produce information requiring the problem-solver to go back to earlier stages to make modifications. This is particularly the case if the response was not as successful as expected.

The right side of Figure 1 lists critical questions to address to conduct an evaluation. During the scanning stage, you must define the problem with sufficient precision to measure it. You will collect baseline data on the nature and scope of the problem during the analysis phase. Virtually every important question to be addressed during analysis will be important during assessment. This is because, during assessment, you want to know if the problem has changed. So data uncovered during analysis become vital baseline information (or "preresponse measures") during assessment.

Fig. 1. The problem-solving process and evaluation

During the response stage, while developing a strategy to reduce the problem, you should also develop an accountability mechanism to be sure the various participants in the response do what they should be doing. As we will see later, one type of evaluation–process–is closely tied to accountability. Thus, while developing a response, it is important to determine how to assess accountability. Also, the type of response has a major influence on how you design the other type of evaluation–impact.

During assessment, you answer the following questions: Did the response occur as planned? Did the problem decline? If so, are there good reasons to believe the decline resulted from the response?

In summary, you begin planning for an evaluation when you take on a problem. The evaluation builds throughout the SARA process, culminates during the assessment, and provides findings that help you determine if you should revisit earlier stages to improve the response. You can use the checklist in Appendix E as a general guide to evaluation throughout the SARA process.

Types of Evaluations

There are two types of evaluations. You should conduct both. As we will see later, they complement each other.

Process Evaluations

Process evaluations ask the following questions: Did the response occur as planned? Did all the response components work? Or, stated more bluntly, Did you do what you said you would do? This is a question of accountability. Let's start with a hypothetical example. A problem-solving team, after a careful analysis, determines that, to curb a street prostitution problem, they will ask the city's traffic engineering department to make a major thoroughfare oneway, and to create several dead-end streets to thwart cruising by "johns." This will be done immediately after a comprehensive crackdown on the prostitutes in the target area. Convicted prostitutes will be given probation under the condition that they do not enter the target area for a year. Finally, a nonprofit organization will help prostitutes who want to leave their line of work gain the necessary skills for legitimate employment. The vice squad, district patrol officers, prosecutor, local judges, probation office, sheriff's department, traffic engineering department, and nonprofit organization all agree to this plan. A process evaluation will determine whether the crackdown occurred and, if so, how many arrests police made; whether the traffic engineering department altered street patterns as planned; and how many prostitutes asked for job skills assistance and found legitimate employment. The process evaluation will also examine whether everything occurred in the planned sequence. If you find that the crackdown occurred after the street alterations, that the police arrested only a fraction of the prostitutes, and that none of the prostitutes sought job skills, then you will suspect that the plan was not fully carried out, nor was it carried out in the specified sequence. You might conclude that the response was a colossal failure. However, the evidence provided gives us no indication of success or failure, because a process evaluation does not answer the question, What happened to the problem?

Impact Evaluations

To determine what happened to the problem, you need an impact evaluation. An impact evaluation asks the following questions: Did the problem decline? If so, did the response cause the decline? Continuing with our prostitution example, let's look at how it might work. During the analysis stage of the problem-solving process, patrol officers and vice detectives conduct a census of prostitutes operating in the target area. They also ask the traffic engineering department to install traffic counters on the major thoroughfare and critical side streets to measure traffic flow. This is done to determine how customers move through the area. The vice squad makes covert video recordings of the target area to document how prostitutes interact with potential customers. All of this is done before the problem-solving team selects a response, and the information gained helps the team to do so. After the response is implemented (though not the planned response, as we have seen), the team decides to repeat these measures to see if the problem has declined. They discover that instead of the 23 prostitutes counted in the first census, only 10 can be found. They also find that there has been a slight decline in traffic on the major thoroughfare on Friday and Saturday nights, but not at other times. However, there has been a substantial decline in side street traffic on Friday and Saturday nights. New covert video recordings show that prostitutes in the area have changed how they approach vehicles, and are acting more cautiously. In short, the team has evidence that the problem has declined after response implementation. So what has caused the problem to decline? You may be tempted to jump right into trying to answer this question, because it will help you determine if you can attribute the decline to the response. However, this question may not be as important as it first appears. After all, if the goal is to reduce or eliminate the problem, and this occurs, what difference does it make what the cause is? The answer is that it does not matter in the least, unless you are interested in using the same response for similar problems. If you have no interest in using the response again, then all that matters is that you have achieved the goal. You can then use the resources devoted to addressing the problem on some more pressing concern. But if you believe you can use the response again, it is very important to determine if the response caused the decline in the problem. Let's assume the prostitution problem-solving team believes the response might be useful for addressing similar problems. The response, though not implemented according to plan, might have caused the decline, but it is also possible that something else caused the decline. There are two reasons the team takes this second possibility seriously. First, the actual response was somewhat haphazard, unlike the planned response. If the planned response had been implemented, the team would have a plausible explanation for the decline. But the jury-rigged nature of the actual response makes it a far less plausible explanation for the decline. Second, the impact evaluation is not particularly strong. Later, we will discuss why this is a weak evaluation, and what can be done to strengthen it.

Interpretation of Process and Impact Evaluations

Process and impact evaluations answer different questions, so their combined results are often highly informative. Table 1 summarizes the information you can glean from both evaluations. As you will see in Appendix E , the interpretation of this table depends on the type of design used for the impact evaluation. For the moment, however, we will assume that the evaluation design can show whether the response caused the problem to decline.

Table 1: Interpreting Results of Process and Impact Evaluations

When a response is implemented as planned (or nearly so), the conclusions are much easier to interpret (cells A and B). When the response is not implemented as planned, we have more difficulty determining what happened, and what to do next (cells C and D). Cell D is particularly troublesome because all you really know is that "we did not do it, and it did not work." Should you try to implement your original plan, or should you start over from scratch? Outcomes that fall into cell C merit further discussion. The decline in the problem means that you could end the problem-solving process and go on to something else. If the problem has declined considerably, this might be satisfactory. If, however, the problem is still too big, then you do not know whether to continue or increase the response (on the assumption that it is working, but more is needed). Alternatively, you could seek a different response (on the assumption that the response is not working, and something else is needed). In addition, you do not know if the response will be useful for similar problems. In short, it is difficult to replicate successes when you do not know why you were successful. The basic lesson is that all assessments should contain both a process and an impact evaluation.

A process evaluation involves comparing the planned response with what actually occurred. Much of this information becomes apparent while managing a problemsolving process. If the vice squad is supposed to arrest prostitutes in the target area, you can determine whether they have from departmental records and discussions with squad members. There will be judgment calls, nevertheless. For example, how many arrests are required? The response plan may call for the arrest of 75 percent of the prostitutes, but only 60 percent are arrested. Whether this is a serious violation of the plan may be difficult to determine. Much of a process evaluation is descriptive (these people did these things, in this order, using these procedures). Nevertheless, numbers can help. In our example, data on traffic volume show where street alterations have changed driving patterns, and these pattern changes are consistent with what was anticipated in the response plan.

In short, a process evaluation tells what happened, when and to whom. Though it does not tell whether the response affected the problem, it is very useful for determining how to interpret impact evaluation results.

Free Bound Copies of the Tool Guides

You may order free bound copies in any of three ways:

Online: Department of Justice COPS Response Center

Email: [email protected]

Phone: 800-421-6770 or 202-307-1480

Allow several days for delivery.

Email sent. Thank you.

Assessing responses to problems.

Send an e-mail with a link to this guide.

Error sending email. Please review your enteries below.

Separate multiple addresses with commas (,)

  • Your Name *

Please limit your note to 200 characters.

  • Translations
  • Privacy Policy
  • POP Projects
  • Learning Center
  • POP Conference

Copyright © 2016 Center for Problem-Oriented Policing

COMMENTS

  1. The SARA Model

    The SARA Model. A commonly used problem-solving method is the SARA model (Scanning, Analysis, Response and Assessment). The SARA model contains the following elements: Scanning: Identifying recurring problems of concern to the public and the police. Identifying the consequences of the problem for the community and the police. Prioritizing those ...

  2. PDF Identifying and Defining Policing Problems

    This guidebook deals with the process of identifying and defining policing problems. Under the most widely adopted police problem-solving model—the SARA (Scanning, Analysis, Response, Assessment) model—the process of identifying and defining policing problems is referred to as the Scanning phase.

  3. PDF A practice guide

    The first stage of the problem-solving process is to identify a problem. But what problem to pick? The police are expected to deal with a wide range of issues. Some of these relate to crime, such as burglary, theft and assaults. Some are not directly to do with crime, such as missing persons, traffic congestion and attempted

  4. Scanning, Analysis, Response, and Assessment

    Method. SARA consists of four stages: [3] Scanning: The officer identifies an issue and determines if it represents a problem that needs to be addressed. Analysis: The officer collects information about the problem from various sources to understand the causes and scope of the problem. Response: The officer uses the information to create and ...

  5. PDF Problem-Oriented Policing: The SARA Model

    POP Course Description. modules also help learners identify when to move from one phase to the next. Correctly identifying the real problem in a community is a critical step in making a lasting impact on neighborhood crime and disorder. Learners explore the importance of assessment, types of evaluations, and nontraditional measures for ...

  6. Problem-Solving and SARA

    This chapter reflects on problem-solving from the analyst perspective and considers the following themes: problem-solving analysis, using the SARA model, and concludes with some ideas about how you can become a problem-solving analyst. ... Analysis is a genuine part of the problem-solving process that can lead to valuable partnerships between ...

  7. PDF Assessing Responses to Problems

    of problem-oriented policing and the problem-solving process. This guide assumes a basic understanding of the SARA problem-solving process (scanning, analysis, response, and assessment), but it requires little or no experience with assessing problem solutions. This guide was written based on the assumption that you have no outside assistance.

  8. Enhancing SARA: a new approach in an increasingly complex world

    Problem oriented policing (POP), commonly referred to as problem-solving in the UK, was first described by Goldstein ( 1979, 1990) and operationalised by Eck and Spelman ( 1987) using the SARA model. SARA is the acronym for Scanning, Analysis, Response and Assessment. It is essentially a rational method to systematically identify and analyse ...

  9. PDF Problem-Solving Tips

    contains information and insights into the process. It will take you step by step through solving problems, offer examples of problem solving from the field, and provide additional resources. ... the SARA model to guide their problem-solving efforts. Although the SARA model is not the only way to approach problem solving, it can serve as a ...

  10. Problem‐oriented policing for reducing crime and disorder: An updated

    In an application of problem solving in Newport News, in which Goldstein acted as a consultant, they developed the SARA model for problem solving. SARA is an acronym representing four steps they suggest police should follow when implementing POP, which will be outlined in Section 2.2. 1

  11. The S.A.R.A. Model

    S.A.R.A. looks to identify and overcome the underlying causes of crime and disorder versus just treating the symptoms. It can be applied to any community problem by implementing each of four steps in the model: Scanning, Analysis, Response, and Assessment. First Step - Scanning. During the scanning phase, law enforcement works with community ...

  12. Center for Problem-Oriented Policing

    The SARA Model . A commonly used problem-solving method is the SARA model (Scanning, Analysis, Response and Assessment). The SARA model contains the following elements: Scanning: Identifying recurring problems of concern to the public and the police. Identifying the consequences of the problem for the community and the police.

  13. PDF Excellence in Problem-Oriented Policing: The 2000 Herman Goldstein

    The preeminent conceptual model of problem solving, known as SARA, grew out of the problem-oriented policing project in Newport News. The acronym SARA stands for scanning, analysis, response, and assessment. This model has become the basis for many police agencies' training curricula and problem-solving efforts.

  14. Identifying and Defining Policing Problems

    Introduction. This Problem-Solving Tools guidebook deals with the process of identifying and defining policing problems. Under the most widely adopted police problem-solving model—the SARA (Scanning, Analysis, Response, Assessment) model—the process of identifying and defining policing problems is referred to as the Scanning phase.

  15. What is the SARA model?

    This problem-solving guide consists ... In the first of 16 short explainers, we look at the SARA model and how it can be applied to problem solving in policing.

  16. Problem-Oriented Policing: The SARA Model

    Problem-Oriented Policing: The SARA Model, an eLearning course, provides learners with a basic awareness and understanding of the fundamental principles of a common approach used by many community policing agencies to identify and solve repeat crime and community problems.The SARA model allows agencies to scan through multiple data sources, conduct a thorough analysis of a problem through the ...

  17. The SARA Problem Solving Model

    The problem-solving approach is a methodical process for reducing the impact of crime and disorder problems in a community. One problem solving model frequently used is the SARA model (Scanning, Analysis, Response, Assessment). Defined below, this four-step process is implemented by the policing agency in partnership with the community. Scanning

  18. Problem-solving policing

    The SARA model helps to apply problem-solving ideas to police practice. It's part of an approach to policing that encourages working creatively and collaboratively with partners and communities experiencing problems. Problem solving provides a process - a tried and tested series of steps to guide and structure efforts to reduce crime and ...

  19. SARA

    A generally used problem-solving method is the SARA (Scan, Analysis, Response and Evaluation) model. The SARA model contains the following elements: Scanning: - Identify repeated problems for the ...

  20. The SARA Model

    The SARA Model. A commonly used problem-solving method is the SARA model (Scanning, Analysis, Response and Assessment). The SARA model contains the following elements: Scanning: Identifying recurring problems of concern to the public and the police. Identifying the consequences of the problem for the community and the police. Prioritizing those ...

  21. Center for Problem-Oriented Policing

    Assessment occurs at the final stage in the SARA problem-solving process.2 It is the culmination of the evaluation process, the time when you draw conclusions about the problem and its solutions. Though assessment is the final stage of both evaluation and problem solving, critical decisions about the evaluation are made throughout the process ...

  22. PDF School-Based Partnerships: A Problem-Solving Strategy

    This crime triangle informs all aspects of the SARA problem solving process.§ This four-step model offers a framework for approaching crime problems and consists of scanning, analysis, response, and assessment:§§ Scanning The scanning phase involves problem identification. Its objectives are to define a basic problem,

  23. Final part 3

    What is a strength of the SARA (for scanning, analysis, response, assessment) problem-solving process? Choose matching definition. a. special-trained in-house personnel b. a sociology professor at a local university c. an economics professor at a local university answer--> all of the above.